logo

Lex Fridman Podcast

Conversations about science, technology, history, philosophy and the nature of intelligence, consciousness, love, and power. Lex is an AI researcher at MIT and beyond. Conversations about science, technology, history, philosophy and the nature of intelligence, consciousness, love, and power. Lex is an AI researcher at MIT and beyond.

Transcribed podcasts: 442
Time transcribed: 44d 12h 13m 31s

This graph shows how many times the word ______ has been mentioned throughout the history of the program.

The following is a conversation with Manolis Kellis.
He's a professor at MIT and head of the MIT Computational Biology Group.
He's interested in understanding the human genome from a computational,
evolutionary, biological, and other cross-disciplinary perspectives.
He has more big impactful papers and awards than I can list.
But most importantly, he's a kind, curious, brilliant
human being and just someone I really enjoy talking to.
His passion for science and life in general is contagious.
The hours honestly flew by and I'm sure we'll talk again on this podcast soon.
Quick summary of the ads.
Three sponsors.
Blinkist, Aidsleep, and Masterclass.
Please consider supporting this podcast by going to blinkist.com slash lex,
aidsleep.com slash lex, and signing up at masterclass.com slash lex.
Click the links, buy the stuff, get the discount.
It's the best way to support this podcast.
If you enjoy this thing, subscribe on YouTube,
review it with five stars on Apple Podcast, support it on Patreon,
or connect with me on Twitter at Lex Freedman.
As usual, I'll do a few minutes of ads now and never any ads in the middle
that can break the flow of the conversation.
This episode is supported by Blinkist, my favorite app for learning new things.
Get it at blinkist.com slash lex for a seven-day free trial
and 25% off afterwards.
Blinkist takes the key ideas from thousands of nonfiction books
and condenses them down into just 15 minutes that you can read or listen to.
I'm a big believer in reading at least an hour every day.
As part of that, I use blinkist every day to try out a book
I may otherwise never have a chance to read.
And in general, it's a great way to broaden your view of the idea landscape out there
and find books that you may want to read more deeply.
With Blinkist, you get unlimited access to read or listen to a massive library
of condensed nonfiction books.
Go to blinkist.com slash lex to try it free for seven days
and save 25% off your new subscription.
That's blinkist.com slash lex.
Blinkist spelled B-L-I-N-K-I-S-T.
This show is also sponsored by ASleep and its PodPro mattress
that you can check out at ASleep.com slash lex to get $200 off.
It controls temperature with an app.
It can cool down to as low as 55 degrees on each side of the bed separately.
Research shows that temperature has a big impact on the quality of our sleep.
Anecdotally, it's been true for me.
It's truly been a game changer.
I love it.
The PodPro is packed with sensors that track heart rate,
heart rate variability, and respiratory rate, showing it all in their app.
The app's health metrics are amazing, but the cooling alone is honestly worth the money.
Check it out at ASleep.com slash lex to get $200 off.
This show is also sponsored by Masterclass.
Sign up at masterclass.com slash lex to get a discount and to support this podcast.
When I first heard about Masterclass, I thought it was too good to be true.
For 180 bucks a year, you get an all-access pass to watch courses from
to list some of my favorites, Chris Hadfield on Space Exploration,
Neil deGrasse Tyson on Scientific Thinking and Communication,
Will Wright, one of my favorite game designers, Carlos Santana,
one of my favorite guitar players, Gary Kasparov, of course,
the greatest chess player of all time.
I'm not biased, Daniel Negrano on poker, and many more.
Chris Hadfield explaining how rockets work and the experience of being
launched into space alone is worth the money.
By the way, you can watch it on basically any device.
Once again, sign up at masterclass.com slash lex to get a discount
and to support this podcast.
And now, here's my conversation with Manolis Kellis.
What to you is the most beautiful aspect of the human genome?
Don't get me started.
So, the first answer is that the beauty of genomes transcends humanity.
So it's not just about the human genome.
Genomes in general are amazingly beautiful.
And again, I'm obviously biased.
So in my view, the way that I like to introduce the human genome
and the way that I like to introduce genomics to my class
is by telling them, you know, we're not the inventors
of the first digital computer.
We are the descendants of the first digital computer.
Basically, life is digital, and that's absolutely beautiful about life.
The fact that at every replication step,
you don't lose any information because that information is digital.
If it was analog, if it was just protein concentrations,
you'd lose it after a few generations.
It would just dissolve away.
And that's what the ancients didn't understand about inheritance.
The first person to understand digital inheritance was Mendel, of course.
And his theory, in fact, stayed in a bookshelf for like 50 years
while Darwin was getting famous about natural selection.
But the missing component was this digital inheritance,
the mechanism of evolution that Mendel had discovered.
So that aspect, in my view, is the most beautiful aspect, but it transcends all of life.
And can you elaborate maybe the inheritance part?
What was the key thing that the ancients didn't understand?
So the very theory of inheritance as discrete units throughout the life of Mendel,
and well after his writing, people thought that his P experiments
were just a little fluke, that they were just a little exception
that would normally not even apply to humans.
That basically what they saw is this continuum of eye color,
this continuum of skin color, this continuum of hair color,
this continuum of height.
And all of these continuums did not fit with a discrete type of inheritance
that Mendel was describing.
But what's unique about genomics and what's unique about the genome
is really that there are two copies and that you get a combination of these.
But for every trait, there are dozens of contributing variables.
And it was only Ronald Fisher in the 20th century
that basically recognized that even five Mendelian traits
would add up to a continuum-like inheritance pattern.
And he wrote a series of papers that still are very relevant today
about this Mendelian inheritance of continuum-like traits.
And I think that was the missing step in inheritance.
So well before the discovery of the structure of DNA,
which is, again, another amazingly beautiful aspect,
the double helix, what I like to call the most noble molecule of our time,
holds within it the secret of that discrete inheritance.
But the conceptualization of discrete elements
is something that precedes that.
So even though it's discrete, when it materializes its own
itself into the actual traits that we see,
it can be continuous and basically arbitrarily rich and complex.
So if you have five genes that contribute to human height
and there aren't five, there's a thousand.
If there's only five genes and you inherit some combination of them
and everyone makes you two inches taller or two inches shorter,
it'll look like a continuum trait, a continuous trait.
But instead of five, there are thousands
and every one of them contributes to less than one millimeter.
We change in height more during the day
than each of these genetic variants contribute.
So by the evening, you're shorter than you walk up with.
Isn't it weird then that we're not more different than we are?
Why are we all so similar if there's so much possibility to be different?
Yeah, so there are selective advantages to being medium.
If you're extremely tall or extremely short,
you run into selective disadvantages.
So you have trouble breathing, you have trouble running,
you have trouble sitting.
If you're too tall, if you're too short,
you might have other selective pressures acting against that.
If you look at natural history of human population,
there's actually selection for height in Northern Europe
and selection against height in Southern Europe.
So there might actually be advantages to actually being not super tall.
And if you look across the entire human population,
for many, many traits, there's a lot of push towards the middle.
A balancing selection is the usual term for selection
that seeks to not be extreme and to have a combination of alleles
that keep recombining.
And if you look at mate selection,
super, super tall people will not tend to marry super, super tall people.
Very often, you see these couples that are compensating for each other.
And the best predictor of the kid's age is very often just take the average
of the two parents and then adjust for sex.
And boom, you get it.
It's extremely heritable.
Let me ask, you kind of took a step back to the genome outside of just humans.
But is there something that you find beautiful
about the human genome specifically?
So I think that genome, if more people understood the beauty of the human genome,
they would be so many fewer wars, so much less anger in the world.
I mean, what's really beautiful about the human genome
is really the variation that teaches us both about individuality and about similarity.
So any two people on the planet are 99.9% identical.
How can you fight with someone who is 99.9% identical to you?
It's just counterintuitive.
And yet, any two siblings of the same parents differ in millions of locations.
So every one of them is basically two to the million unique from any pair of parents,
let alone any two random parents on the planet.
So that's, I think, something that teaches us about sort of the nature of humanity in many ways,
that every one of us is as unique as any star and way more unique in actually many ways.
And yet, we're all brothers and sisters.
Yeah, just like stars, most of it is just fusion reactions.
Yeah, you only have a few parameters to describe stars.
Yeah, exactly.
You know, mass size, initial size, and stage of life.
Whereas for humans, it's thousands of parameters scattered across the genome.
So the other thing that makes humans unique, the other things that makes inheritance unique
in humans is that most species inherit things vertically.
Basically, instinct is a huge part of their behavior.
The way that, you know, I mean, with my kids, we've been watching this nest of birds
with two little eggs, you know, outside our window for the last few months,
for the last few weeks as they've been growing.
And there's so much behavior that's hard coded.
Birds don't just learn as they grow.
They don't, you know, there's no culture.
Like a bird that's born in Boston will be the same as a bird that's born in California.
So there's not as much inheritance of ideas, of customs.
A lot of it is hard coding in their genome.
What's really beautiful about the human genome is that if you take a person from today
and you place them back in ancient Egypt, or if you take a person from ancient Egypt
and you place them here today, they will grow up to be completely normal.
That is not genetics.
This is the other type of inheritance in humans.
So on one hand, we have genetic inheritance, which is vertical from your parents down.
On the other hand, we have horizontal inheritance,
which is the ideas that are built up at every generation are horizontally transmitted.
And the huge amount of time that we spend in educating ourselves,
a concept known as neoteny,
neo for newborn and then teni for holding.
So if you look at humans, I mean, the little birds that were, you know, eggs two weeks ago,
and now one of them has already flown off, the other one's ready to fly off.
In two weeks, they're ready to just fend for themselves.
Humans, 16 years, 18 years, 24, getting out of college.
I'm still learning.
So that's so fascinating, this picture of vertical and horizontal.
When you talk about the horizontal, is it in the realm of ideas?
Exactly.
Okay.
So it's the actual social interactions.
That's exactly right.
That's exactly right.
So basically the concept of neoteny is that you spend acquiring characteristics from your environment
in an extremely malleable state of your brain and the wiring of your brain
for a long period of your life.
Compared to primates, we are useless.
You take any primate at seven weeks, and in human at seven weeks, we lose the battle.
But at 18 years, you know, all better off.
Like we basically, our brain continues to develop in an extremely malleable form till very late.
And this is what allows education.
This is what allows the person from Egypt to do extremely well now.
And the reason for that is that the wiring of our brain and the development of that wiring is actually delayed.
So, you know, the longer you delay that, the more opportunity you have to pass on knowledge,
to pass on concepts, ideals, ideas from the parents to the child.
And what's really absolutely beautiful about humans today is that that lateral transfer of ideas and culture
is not just from uncles and aunts and teachers at school, but it's from Wikipedia and review articles on the web
and thousands of journals that are sort of putting out information for free
and podcasts and videocasts and all of that stuff, where you can basically learn about any topic
pretty much everything that would be in any super advanced textbook in a matter of days.
Instead of having to go to the library of Alexandria and sail there to read three books
and then sail for another few days to get to Athens and et cetera, et cetera, et cetera.
So, the democratization of knowledge and the spread, the speed of spread of knowledge
is what defines, I think, the human inheritance pattern.
So, you sound excited about it.
Are you also a little bit afraid or are you more excited by the power of this kind of
distributed spread of information?
So, you put it very kindly that most people are kind of using the internet and looking Wikipedia,
reading articles, reading papers, and so on, but if we're honest, most people online,
especially when they're younger, probably looking at five-second clips on TikTok
or whatever the new social network is, are you, given this power of horizontal inheritance,
are you optimistic or a little bit pessimistic about this new effect of the internet
and democratization of knowledge on our, what would you call this?
Like, would you use the term genome, by the way, for this?
Yeah, I think we use the genome to talk about DNA, but very often we say, I mean,
I'm Greek, so people ask me, hey, what's in the Greek genome?
And I'm like, well, yeah, what's in the Greek genome is both our genes and also our ideas
and our ideals and our culture.
The poetic meaning of the word.
Exactly, exactly.
So, I think that there's a beauty to the democratization of knowledge, the fact that
you can reach as many people as any other person on the planet and it's not who you are,
it's really your ideas that matter, is a beautiful aspect of the internet.
The, I think there's, of course, a danger of my ignorance is as important as your expertise.
The fact that with this democratization comes the abolishment of respecting expertise.
Just because you spent, you know, 10,000 hours of your life studying, I don't know, human brain circuitry,
why should I trust you, I'm just going to make up my own theories and they'll be just as good as yours,
is an attitude that sort of counteracts the beauty of the democratization.
And I think that within our educational system and within the upbringing of our children,
we have to not only teach them knowledge, but we have to teach them the means to get to knowledge.
And that, you know, it's very similar to sort of you fish, you catch a fish for a man for one day, you fed them for one day,
you teach them how to fish, you fed them for the rest of their life.
So, instead of just gathering the knowledge they need for any one task, we can just tell them,
all right, here's how you Google it, here's how you figure out what's real and what's not,
here's how you check the sources, here's how you form a basic opinion for yourself.
And I think that inquisitive nature is paramount to being able to sort through this huge wealth of knowledge.
So, you need a basic educational foundation based on which you can then add on the sort of domain-specific knowledge,
but that basic educational foundation should just not just be knowledge,
but it should also be epistemology, the way to acquire knowledge.
I'm not sure any of us know how to do that in this modern day.
We're actually learning, one of the big surprising things to me about the coronavirus, for example,
is that Twitter has been one of the best sources of information,
basically like building your own network of experts, as opposed to the traditional centralized expertise of the WHO and the CDC
or maybe any one particular respectable person at the top of a department or some kind of institution.
You instead look at 10, 20 hundreds of people, some of whom are young kids with just that are incredibly good at aggregating data
and plotting and visualizing that data.
That's been really surprising to me.
I don't know what to make of it.
I don't know how that matures into something stable.
I don't know if you have ideas.
If you were to try to explain to your kids where should you go to learn about coronavirus, what would you say?
It's such a beautiful example.
And I think the current pandemic and the speed at which the scientific community has moved in the current pandemic,
exemplifies this horizontal transfer and the speed of horizontal transfer of information.
The fact that the genome was first sequenced in early January, the first sample was obtained December 29, 2019,
a week after the publication of the first genome sequence of Moderna had already finalized its vaccine design and was moving to production.
This is phenomenal, the fact that we go from not knowing what the heck is killing people in Wuhan to, wow, it's SARS-CoV-2
and here's the set of genes, here's the genome, here's the sequence, here are the polymorphisms, etc.
In the matter of weeks is phenomenal.
In that incredible pace of transfer of knowledge, there have been many mistakes.
Some of those mistakes may have been politically motivated, other mistakes may have just been innocuous errors,
others may have been misleading the public for the greater good, such as don't wear masks because we don't want the mask to run out.
That was very silly in my view and a very big mistake.
But the spread of knowledge from the scientific community was phenomenal
and some people will point out to bogus articles that snuck in and made the front page.
Yeah, they did, but within 24 hours they were debunked and went out of the front page
and I think that's the beauty of science today.
The fact that it's not, oh, knowledge is fixed.
It's the ability to embrace that nothing is permanent when it comes to knowledge,
that everything is the current best hypothesis and the current best model that best fits the current data
and the willingness to be wrong, the expectation that we're going to be wrong
and the celebration of success based on how long was I not proven wrong for
rather than, wow, I was exactly right because no one is going to be exactly right with partial knowledge
but the arc towards perfection I think is so much more important than how far you are in your first step
and I think that's what sort of the current pandemic has taught us,
but yeah, no, of course, we're going to make mistakes, but at least we're going to learn from those mistakes
and become better and learn better and spread information better.
So if I were to answer the question of where would you go to learn about coronavirus?
First textbook, it all starts with a textbook.
Just open up a chapter on virology and how coronaviruses work,
then some basic epidemiology and sort of how pandemics have worked in the past.
What are the basic principles surrounding these first wave, second wave?
Why do they even exist?
Then understanding about growth, understanding about the R0 and RT at various time points
and then understanding the means of spread, how it spreads from person to person,
then how does it get into your cells from when it gets into the cells?
What are the paths that it takes?
What are the cell types that express the particular ACE2 receptor?
What are the immune systems interacting with the virus?
And once your immune system launches it defense, how is that helping or actually hurting your health?
What about the cytokine storm?
What are most people dying from?
Why are the comorbidities and these risk factors even applying?
What makes obese people respond more or elderly people respond more to the virus
while kids are completely very often not even aware that they're spreading it?
I think there's some basic questions that you would start from
and then I'm sorry to say but Wikipedia is pretty awesome.
Google is pretty awesome.
It used to be a time maybe five years ago.
I forget when but people kind of made fun of Wikipedia for being an unreliable source.
I never quite understood it.
I thought from the early days it was pretty reliable.
They're better than a lot of the alternatives.
At this point it's kind of like a solid accessible survey paper on every subject ever.
There's an ascertainment bias and a writing bias.
So I think this is related to people saying, oh, so many nature papers are wrong.
And they're like, why would you publish in nature?
So many nature papers are wrong.
And my answer is no, no, no.
So many nature papers are scrutinized.
Just because more of them are being proven wrong than in other articles is actually evidence
that they're actually better papers overall because they're being scrutinized at a rate
much higher than any other journal.
So if you basically judge Wikipedia by not the initial content but by the number of revisions,
then of course it's going to be the best source of knowledge eventually.
It's still very superficial.
You then have to go into the review papers, et cetera, et cetera, et cetera.
But I mean, for most scientific topics, it's extremely superficial.
But it is quite authoritative because it is the place that everybody likes to criticize as being wrong.
You say that it's superficial.
And a lot of topics that I've studied a lot of, I find it, I don't know if superficial is the right word.
Because superficial kind of implies that it's not correct.
No, no.
I don't mean any implication of it not being correct.
It's just superficial.
It's basically only scratching the surface.
For depth, you don't go to Wikipedia and you go to the review articles.
But it can be profound in the way that articles rarely...
One of the frustrating things to me about certain computer science, like in the machine learning world,
articles, they don't as often take the bigger picture view.
There's a kind of data set and you show that it works and you kind of show that here's an architectural thing that creates an improvement and so on and so forth.
But you don't say, well, what does this mean for the nature of intelligence for future data sets we haven't even thought about?
Or if you were trying to implement this...
If we took this data set of 100,000 examples and scale it to 100 billion examples with this method,
look at the bigger picture, which is what a Wikipedia article would actually try to do,
which is what does this mean in the context of the broad field of computer vision or something like that?
I agree with you completely, but it depends on the topic.
I mean, for some topics, there's been a huge amount of work.
For other topics, it's just a stub.
I got it.
Well, yeah, actually, which we'll talk on, genomics was not great.
Yeah, it's very shallow.
It's not wrong, it's just shallow.
Every time I criticize something, I should feel partly responsible.
Basically, if more people from my community went there and edited, it would not be shallow.
It's just that there's different modes of communication in different fields.
And in some fields, the experts have embraced Wikipedia, in other fields, it's relegated.
And perhaps the reason is that if it was any better to start with, people would invest more time.
But if it's not great to start with, then you need a few initial pioneers who will basically go in and say,
enough, we're just going to fix that.
And then I think it'll catch on much more.
So if it's okay before we go on to genomics, can we linger a little bit longer on the beauty of the human genome?
You've given me a few notes.
What else do you find beautiful about the human genome?
So the last aspect of what makes the human genome unique, in addition to the similarity and the differences and the individuality,
is that, so very early on, people would basically say, oh, you don't do that experiment in human.
You have to learn about that in fly.
Or you have to learn about that in yeast first, or in mouse first, or in a primate first.
And the human genome was in fact relegated to sort of, oh, the last place that you're going to go to learn something new.
That has dramatically changed.
And the reason that changed is human genetics.
We are these species in the planet that's the most studied right now.
It's embarrassing to say that.
But this was not the case a few years ago.
It used to be, you know, first viruses, then bacteria, then yeast, then the fruit fly and the worm, then the mouse.
And eventually, human was very far last.
So it's embarrassing that it took us this long to focus on it?
It's embarrassing that the model organisms have been taken over because of the power of human genetics.
That right now, it's actually simpler to figure out the phenotype of something by mining this massive amount of human data than by going back to any of the other species.
And the reason for that is that if you look at the natural variation that happens in a population of seven billion,
you basically have a mutation in almost every nucleotide.
So every nucleotide you want to perturb, you can go find a living, breathing human being
and go test the function of that nucleotide by sort of searching the database and finding that person.
Wait, why is that embarrassing? It's a beautiful data set.
It's embarrassing for the model organism.
For the flies.
Yeah, exactly.
I mean, do you feel on a small tangent, is there something of value in the genome of a fly
and other of these model organisms that you miss that you wish we would be looking at deeper?
So directed perturbation, of course.
So I think the place where humans are still lagging is the fact that in an animal model,
you can go and say, well, let me knock out this gene completely and let me knock out these three genes completely.
And the moment you get into combinatorics, it's something you can't do in the human
because there just simply aren't enough humans on the planet.
And again, let me be honest, we haven't sequenced all seven billion people.
It's not like we have every mutation, but we know that there's a carrier out there.
So if you look at the trend and the speed with which human genetics has progressed,
we can now find thousands of genes involved in human cognition, in human psychology,
in the emotions and the feelings that we used to think are uniquely learned.
Turns out there's a genetic basis to a lot of that.
So the human genome has continued to elucidate through these studies of genetic variation
so many different processes that we previously thought were something like free will.
Free will is this beautiful concept that humans have had for a long time.
In the end, it's just a bunch of chemical reactions happening in your brain
and the particular abundance of receptors that you have this day based on what you ate yesterday
or that you have been wired with based on your parents and your upbringing, etc.
Determines a lot of that quote-unquote free will component to sort of narrow slices.
So on that point, how much freedom do you think we have to escape the constraints of our genome?
You're making it sound like more and more we're discovering that our genome
actually has a lot of the story already encoded into it.
How much freedom do we have?
So let me describe what that freedom would look like.
That freedom would be my saying,
ooh, I'm going to resist the urge to eat that apple because I choose not to.
But there are chemical receptors that made me not resist the urge to prove my individuality
and my free will by resisting the apple.
So then the next question is, well, maybe now I'll resist the urge to resist the apple
and I'll go for the chocolate instead to prove my individuality.
But then what about those other receptors that might be all encoded in there?
So it's kicking the bucket down the road and basically saying,
well, your choice may have actually been driven by other things that you actually are not choosing.
So that's why it's very hard to answer that question.
Well, it's hard to know what to do with that.
I mean, if the genome has...
If there's not much freedom, it's...
It's the butterfly effect.
It's basically that in the short term, you can predict something extremely well
by knowing the current state of the system.
But a few steps down, it's very hard to predict based on the current knowledge.
Is that because the system is truly free?
When I look at weather patterns, I can predict the next 10 days.
Is it because the weather has a lot of freedom?
And after 10 days, it chooses to do something else?
Or is it because, in fact, the system is fully deterministic
and there's just a slightly different magnetic field of the earth,
slightly more energy arriving from the sun,
a slightly different spin of the gravitational pull of Jupiter
that is now causing all kinds of tides and slight deviation of the moon, etc.
Maybe all of that can be fully modeled.
Maybe the fact that China is emitting a little more carbon today
is actually going to affect the weather in Egypt in three weeks.
And all of that could be fully modeled.
In the same way, if you take a complete view of a human being now,
I model everything about you,
the question is, can I predict your next step? Probably.
But at how far?
And if it's a little further, is that because of stochasticity
and sort of chaos properties of unpredictability of beyond a certain level,
or was that actually true free will?
Yeah, so the number of variables might be so,
you might need to build an entire universe to be able to model.
To simulate a human, and then maybe that human will be fully simulatable,
but maybe aspects of free will will exist,
and where is that free will coming from?
Is it still coming from the same neurons,
or maybe from a spirit inhabiting these neurons?
But again, it's very difficult empirically to evaluate where does free will begin
and chemical reactions and electric signals.
So on that topic, let me ask the most absurd question
that most MIT faculty roll their eyes on.
But what do you think about the simulation hypothesis and the idea that we live in a simulation?
I think it's completely BS.
There's no empirical evidence.
Absolutely not.
Not in terms of empirical evidence or not,
but in terms of thought experiment,
does it help you think about the universe?
I mean, so if you look at the genome,
it's encoding a lot of the information that is required
to create some of the beautiful human complexity that we see around us.
It's an interesting thought experiment.
How much parameters do we need to have in order to model this full human experience?
If we were to build a video game,
how hard it would be to build a video game
that's convincing enough and fun enough
and has consistent laws of physics, all that stuff.
So it's not interesting to use the thought experiment.
I mean, it's cute, but it's Occam's razor.
I mean, what's more realistic,
the fact that you're actually a machine or that you're a person,
what's the fact that all of my experiences exist inside the chemical molecules that I have
or that somebody's actually simulating all that?
Well, you did refer to humans as a digital computer earlier.
Of course, of course.
It's kind of a machine, right?
I know, but I think the probability of all that is nil
and let the machines wake me up and just terminate me now if it's not.
I challenge you machines.
They're going to wait a little bit to see what you're going to do next.
It's fun to watch, especially the clever humans.
What's the difference to you between the way a computer stores information
and the human genome stores information?
So you also have roots and your work.
Would you say when you introduce yourself at a bar?
It depends who I'm talking to.
Would you say it's computation biology?
Do you reveal your expertise in computers?
It depends who I'm talking to.
I mean, basically, if I meet someone who's in computers,
I'll say, oh, I mean, professor in computer science.
If I meet someone who's in engineering, I say computer science and electrical engineering.
If I meet someone in biology, I'll say, hey, I work in genomics.
If I meet someone in medicine, I'm like, hey, I work on genetics.
You're a fun person to meet at a bar.
I got you.
No, no, but I'm going to try to say is that I don't...
I mean, there's no single attribute that I will define myself as.
There's a few things I know.
There's a few things I study.
There's a few of the things I have degrees on
and there's a few things that I grant degrees in.
And I publish papers across the whole gamut, the whole spectrum
of computation to biology, et cetera.
I mean, the complete answer is that I use computer science to understand biology.
So I develop methods in AI and machine learning statistics and algorithms, et cetera.
But the ultimate goal of my career is to really understand biology.
These things don't advance our understanding of biology.
I'm not as fascinated by them.
Although there are some beautiful computational problems by themselves,
I've sort of made it my mission to apply the power of computer science
to truly understand the human genome, health, disease, you know,
and the whole gamut of how our brain works, our body works and all of that,
which is so fascinating.
So there's not an equivalent sort of complementary dream
of understanding human biology in order to create an artificial life,
an artificial brain, an artificial intelligence
that supersedes the intelligence and the capabilities of us humans.
It's an interesting question. It's a fascinating question.
So understanding the human brain is undoubtedly coupled to how do we make better AI
because so much of AI has, in fact, been inspired by the brain.
It may have taken 50 years since the early days of neural networks
till we have, you know, all of these amazing progress
that we've seen with, you know, deep belief networks
and, you know, all of these advances in Go, in chess, in image synthesis,
in deep fakes, in you name it.
But the underlying architecture is very much inspired by the human brain,
which actually posits a very, very interesting question.
Why are neural networks performing so well?
And they perform amazingly well.
Is it because they can simulate any possible function?
And the answer is no, no, they simulate a very small number of functions.
Is it because they can simulate every possible function in the universe?
And that's where it gets interesting.
The answer is actually, yeah, a little closer to that.
And here's where it gets really fun.
If you look at human brain and human cognition,
it didn't evolve in a vacuum.
It evolved in a world with physical constraints,
like the world that inhabits us.
It is the world that we inhabit.
And if you look at our senses, what do they perceive?
They perceive different, you know, parts of the electromagnetic spectrum.
You know, the hearing is just different movements in air.
The touch, et cetera.
I mean, all of these things, we've built intuitions for the physical world that inhabit.
And our brains and the brains of all animals evolved for that world.
And the AI systems that we have built happen to work well with images
of the type that we encounter in the physical world that we inhabit.
Whereas if you just take noise and you add random signal
that doesn't match anything in our world, neural networks will not do as well.
And that actually basically has this whole loop around this,
which is this was designed by studying our own brain,
which was evolved for our own world.
And they happen to do well in our own world.
And they happen to make the same types of mistakes that humans make many, many times.
And of course, you can engineer images by adding just the right amount of, you know,
sort of pixel deviations to make a zebra look like a bamboo and stuff like that.
Or like a table.
But ultimately, the undocked images, at least,
are very often, you know, mistaken, I don't know, between muffins and dogs, for example,
in the same way that humans make those mistakes.
So it's, you know, there's no doubt in my view that the more we understand about the tricks
that our human brain has evolved to understand the physical world around us,
the more we will be able to bring new computational primitives in our AI systems
to again better understand not just the world around us,
but maybe even the world inside us and maybe even the computational problems
that arise from new types of data that we haven't been exposed to,
but are yet inhabiting the same universe that we live in
with the very tiny little subset of functions from all possible mathematical functions.
Yeah, and that small subset of functions, all that matters for us humans, really.
It's all that has mattered so far, and even within our scientific realm,
it's all that seems to continue to matter.
But, I mean, I always like to think about our senses
and how much of the physical world around us we perceive.
And if you look at the LIGO experiment of the last, you know, year and a half,
has been all over the news, what did LIGO do?
It created a new sense for human beings,
a sense that has never been sensed in the history of our planet.
Gravitational waves have been traversing the Earth
since its creation a few billion years ago.
Life has evolved senses to sense things that were never before sensed.
Light was not perceived by early life, no one cared.
And eventually, photoreceptors evolved,
and, you know, the ability to sense colors
by sort of catching different parts of that electromagnetic spectrum.
And hearing evolved, and touch evolved, etc.
But no organism evolved a way to sense neutrinos floating through Earth
or gravitational waves flowing through Earth, etc.
And I find it so beautiful in the history of not just humanity,
but life on the planet,
that we are now able to capture additional signals from the physical world
than we ever knew before.
And axions, for example, have been all over the news in the last few weeks.
The concept that we can capture and perceive more of that physical world
is as exciting as the fact that we were blind to it is traumatizing before.
Because that also tells us, you know, we're in 2020.
Picture yourself in 2020. What new senses might we discover?
Could it be that we're missing nine-tenths of physics?
There's a lot of physics out there that we're just blind to, completely oblivious to it.
And yet they're permeating us all the time.
It might be right in front of us.
So when you're thinking about premonitions, a lot of that is ascertainment bias.
Every now and then, you're like, oh, I remember my friend,
and then my friend doesn't appear, and I'll forget that I remembered my friend.
But every now and then, my friend will actually appear.
I'm like, oh, my God, I thought about you a minute ago.
You just called me. That's amazing.
So, you know, some of that is this, but some of that might be that there are,
within our brain, sensors for waves that we emit that we're not even aware of.
And this whole concept of when I hug my children, there's such an emotional transfer there
that we don't comprehend.
I mean, sure, yeah, of course, we're all like hardwired for all kinds of touchy-feely things,
between parents and kids, it's beautiful, between partners, it's beautiful, et cetera.
But then there are intangible aspects of human communication
that I don't think it's unfathomable that our brain has actually evolved ways and sensors for it
that we just don't capture.
We don't understand the function of the vast majority of our neurons.
And maybe our brain is already sensing it.
But even worse, maybe our brain is not sensing it at all.
And we're oblivious to this until we build a machine that suddenly is able to sort of capture
so much more of what's happening in the natural world.
So what you're saying is physics is going to discover a sensor for love.
And maybe dogs are off-scale for that.
And we've been oblivious to it the whole time because we didn't have the right sensor.
And now you're going to have a little wrist that says, oh my god, I feel all this love in the house.
I sense these disturbance in the forest.
It's all around us.
And cats will have zero.
None.
None.
None.
But let's take a step back to our unfortunate place.
To one of the 400 topics that we had actually planned for.
But to our sad time in 2020 when we only have just a few sensors and very primitive early computers.
So in your, you have a foot in computer science and a foot in biology.
In your sense, how do computers represent information differently than like the genome or biological systems?
So first of all, let me correct that no, we're in an amazing time in 2020.
Computer science is totally awesome.
And physics is totally awesome.
And we have understood so much of the natural world than ever before.
So I am extremely grateful and feeling extremely lucky to be living in the time that we are.
Because, you know, first of all, who knows when the asteroid will hit.
And second, you know, of all times in humanity, this is probably the best time to be a human being.
And this might actually be the best place to be a human being.
So anyway, you know, for anyone who loves science, this is, this is it.
This is awesome.
It's a great time.
At the same time, just a quick comment.
All I meant is that if you look several hundred years from now and we end up somehow not destroying ourselves,
people will probably look back at this time in computer science and at your work of Manos and MIT.
Infantile.
Infantile and silly and how ignorant it all was.
I like the joke very often with my students that, you know, we've written so many paper, we've published so much,
we've been citing so much and every single time I tell my students, you know, the best is ahead of us.
What we're working on now is the most exciting thing I've ever worked on.
So in a way, I do have this sense of, yeah, even the papers I wrote 10 years ago, they were awesome at the time.
But I'm so much more excited about where we're heading now.
And I don't mean to minimize any of the stuff we've done in the past, but, you know, there's just this sense of excitement about what you're working on now.
That as soon as the paper is submitted, it's like, ugh, it's old.
Like, you know, I can't talk about that anymore.
At the same time, you're not, you probably are not going to be able to predict what are the most impactful papers and ideas.
When people look back 200 years from now at your work, what would be the most exciting papers?
And it may very well be not the thing that you expected or the things you got awards for or, you know.
And this might be true in some fields.
I don't know. I feel slightly differently about it in our field.
I feel that I kind of know what are the important ones.
And there's a very big difference between what the press picks up on and what's actually fundamentally important for the field.
And I think for the fundamentally important ones, we kind of have a pretty good idea what they are.
And it's hard to sometimes get the press excited about the fundamental advances.
But, you know, we take what we get and celebrate what we get and sometimes, you know, one of our papers which was in a minor journal
made the front page of Reddit and suddenly had like hundreds of thousands of views.
Even though it was in a minor journal because, you know, somebody pitched it the right way that it suddenly caught everybody's attention.
Whereas other papers that are sort of truly fundamental, you know, we have a hard time getting the editors even excited about them
when so many hundreds of people are already using the results and building upon them.
So I do appreciate that there's a discrepancy between the perception and the perceived success and the awards that you get for various papers.
But I think that fundamentally I know that, you know, some paper.
So when you're right, you're most proud.
See now you just, you trapped yourself.
I mean, is there a line of work that you have a sense is really powerful that you've done today?
You've done so much work in so many directions, which is interesting.
Is there something where you think is quite special?
I mean, it's like asking me to say which of my three children I love best.
I mean, exactly.
And it's such a gimme question that is so, so difficult not to brag about the awesome work that my team and my students have done.
And I'll just mention a few off the top of my head.
I mean, basically there's a few landmark papers that I think have shaped my scientific path.
And, you know, I like to somehow describe it as a linear continuation of one thing led to another and led to another led to another.
And, you know, it kind of all started with skip, skip, skip, skip, skip.
Let me try to start somewhere in the middle.
So my first PhD paper was the first comparative analysis of multiple species.
So multiple complete genomes.
So for the first time, we basically developed a concept of genome-wide evolutionary signatures.
The fact that you could look across the entire genome and understand how things evolve.
And from these signatures of evolution, you could go back and study any one region and say that's a protein-coding gene.
That's an RNA gene.
That's a regulatory motif.
That's a, you know, binding site and so on and so forth.
So, I'm sorry, so comparing different species.
Species of the same.
So it takes human, mouse, rat and dog.
You know, they're all animals.
They're all mammals.
They're all performing similar functions with their heart, with their brain, with their lungs, et cetera, et cetera, et cetera.
So there's many functional elements that make us uniquely mammalian.
And those mammalian elements are actually conserved.
99% of our genome does not code for protein.
1% codes for protein.
The other 99%, we frankly didn't know what it does until we started doing this comparative genomic studies.
So basically, these series of papers in my career have basically first developed that concept of evolutionary signatures
and then applied them to yeast, applied them to flies, applied them to four mammals, applied them to 17 fungi,
applied them to 12 susophila species, applied them to then 29 mammals and now 200 mammals.
So, sorry, so can we...
So the evolutionary signatures, it seems like it's such a fascinating idea.
And we're probably going to linger in your early PhD work for two hours.
But how can you reveal something interesting about the genome by looking at the multiple species and looking at the evolutionary signatures?
Yeah, so you basically align the matching regions.
So everything evolved from a common ancestor way, way back.
And mammals evolved from a common ancestor about 60 million years back.
So after the meteor that killed off the dinosaurs landed near Machu Picchu, we know the crater.
It didn't allegedly land.
That was the aliens.
No, just slightly north of Machu Picchu, in the Gulf of Mexico, there's a giant hole that meteor...
By the way, is that definitive?
Have people conclusively figured out what killed the dinosaurs?
I think so.
So it was a meteor?
Well, you know, volcanic activity, all kinds of other stuff is coinciding, but the meteor is pretty unique.
It's also terrifying.
We still have a lot of 2020 left.
So if anything...
No, no, but think about it this way.
So the dinosaurs ruled the Earth for 175 million years.
Humans have been around for what?
Less than one million years?
If you're super generous about what you call humans?
And you include chimps, basically?
So we are just getting warmed up, and we've ruled the planet much more ruthlessly than Tyrannosaurus rex.
Tyrannosaurus rex had much less of an environmental impact than we did.
And if you give us another 174 million years, humans will look very different if we make it that far.
So I think dinosaurs basically are much more of life history on Earth than we are in all respects.
But look at the bright side.
When they were killed off, another life form emerged, mammals.
And that's that whole evolutionary branching that's happened.
So you kind of have...
When you have these evolutionary signatures, is there basically a map of how the genome changed throughout?
Yeah, exactly.
And now you can go back to this early mammal that was hiding in caves, and you can basically ask,
what happened after the dinosaurs were wiped out?
A ton of evolutionary niches opened up, and the mammals started populating all of these niches.
And in that diversification, there was room for expansion of new types of functions.
So some of them populated the air with bats flying, a new evolution of light.
Some populated the oceans with dolphins and whales going off to swim, etc.
But we all are fundamentally mammals.
So you can take the genomes of all these species and align them on top of each other.
And basically create nucleotide resolution correspondences.
What my PhD work showed is that when you do that, when you line up species on top of each other,
you can see that within protein-coding genes, there's a particular pattern of evolution that is dictated
by the level at which evolutionary selection acts.
If I'm coding for a protein and I change the third code on position of a triplet that codes for that amino acid,
the same amino acid will be encoded.
So that basically means that any kind of mutation that preserves that translation, that is invariant
to that ultimate functional assessment that evolution will give, is tolerated.
So for any function that you're trying to achieve, there's a set of sequences that encode it.
You can now look at the mapping, the graph isomorphism, if you wish,
between all of the possible DNA encodings of a particular function and that function.
And instead of having just that exact sequence at the protein level,
you can think of the set of protein sequences that all fulfill the same function.
What's evolution doing?
Evolution has two components.
One component is random, blind, and stupid mutation.
The other component is super smart, ruthless selection.
That's my mom calling from Greece.
Yes, I might be a fully grown man, but I am a Greek.
Did you just cancel the call?
I know, I'm in trouble.
She's going to be calling the cops.
I'm going to edit this clip out and send it to her.
So there's a lot of encoding for the same kind of function.
So you now have this mapping between all of the set of sequences that can all encode the same function.
What evolutionary signatures does is that it basically looks at the shape of that distribution of sequences
that all encode the same thing.
And based on that shape, you can basically say,
ooh, proteins have a very different shape than RNA structures, than regulatory motifs, etc.
So just by scanning a sequence, ignoring the sequence, and just looking at the patterns of change,
I'm like, wow, this thing is evolving like a protein, and that thing is evolving like a motif, and that thing is evolving.
So that's exactly what we just did for COVID.
So our paper that we posted in a bioarchive about coronavirus basically took this concept of evolutionary signatures
and applied it on the SARS-CoV-2 genome that is responsible for the COVID-19 pandemic.
And comparing it to 44 Sarbacovirus species.
So this is the beta.
What word did you just use, Sarbacovirus?
Sarbacovirus, the SARS-related beta coronavirus.
It's a portmanteau of a bunch.
So that's a family of viruses.
Yeah.
How big is that family, by the way?
We have 44 species that...
It's 44 species in the family.
Yeah.
Virus is a clever bunch.
No, no, but there's just 44, and again, we don't call them species in viruses, we call them strains.
But anyway, there's 44 strains, and that's a tiny little subset of maybe another 50 strains that are just far too distantly related.
Most of those only infect bats, as the host, and a subset of only four or five have ever infected humans.
And we basically took all of those and we aligned them in the same exact way that we've aligned mammals.
And then we looked at what proteins are, which of the currently hypothesized genes for the coronavirus genome are in fact evolving like proteins and which ones are not.
And what we found is that ORF-10, the last little open reading frame, the last little gene in the genome, is bogus.
That's not a protein at all.
What is it?
It's an RNA structure.
That doesn't have a...
It doesn't get translated into amino acids.
So it's important to narrow down to basically discover what's useful and what's not.
Exactly.
Basically, what is even the set of genes?
The other thing that these evolutionary signatures showed is that within ORF-3A lies a tiny little additional gene encoded within the other gene.
So you can translate a DNA sequence in three different reading frames.
If you start in the first one, it's ATG, etc.
If you start on the second one, it's TGC, etc.
And there's a gene within a gene.
So there's a whole other protein that we didn't know about that might be super important.
So we don't even know the building blocks of SARS-CoV-2.
So if we want to understand coronavirus biology and eventually find it successfully, we need to even have the set of genes.
And these evolutionary signatures that are developed in my PhD work, we just recently used.
Let's run with that tangent for a little bit if it's okay.
Can we talk about COVID-19 a little bit more?
What's your sense about the genome, the proteins, the functions that we understand about COVID-19?
Where do we stand in your sense?
What are the big open problems?
And also, you kind of said it's important to understand what are the important proteins and why is that important?
So what else does the comparison of these species tell us?
What it tells us is how fast are things evolving?
It tells us about at what level is the acceleration or deceleration pedal set for every one of these proteins?
So the genome has, you know, 30-some genes.
Some genes evolve super, super fast. Others evolve super, super slow.
If you look at the polymerase gene that basically replicates the genome, that's a super slow-evolving one.
If you look at the nucleocapsid protein, that's also super slow-evolving.
If you look at the spike-1 protein, this is the part of the spike protein that actually touches the H2 receptor
and then enables the virus to attach to your cells.
That's the thing that gives it that visual...
Yeah, the corona look, basically.
So basically, the spike protein sticks out of the virus and there's a first part of the protein, S1,
which basically attaches to the H2 receptor and then S2 is the latch that sort of pushes and channels the fusion of the membranes
and then the incorporation of the viral RNA inside our cells,
which then gets translated into all of these 30 proteins.
So the S1 protein is evolving ridiculously fast.
So if you look at the stop versus gas pedal, the gas pedal is all the way down.
Orph 8 is also evolving super fast and Orph 6 is evolving super fast.
We have no idea what they do. We have some idea, but nowhere near what S1 is.
So what the...
Isn't that terrifying that S1 is evolving? That means that's a really useful function.
And if it's evolving fast, doesn't that mean new strains can be created where it does something?
That means that it's searching for how to match, how to best match the host.
So basically anything in general, in evolution, if you look at genomes,
anything that's contacting the environment is evolving much faster than anything that's internal.
And the reason is that the environment changes.
So if you look at the evolution of the cervical viruses,
the S1 protein has evolved very rapidly because it's attaching to different hosts each time.
We think of them as bats, but there's thousands of species of bats.
And to go from one species of bat to another species of bat, you have to adjust
S1 to the new ACE2 receptor that you're going to be facing in that new species.
Sorry, quick tangent.
Is it fascinating to you that viruses are doing this?
I mean, it feels like they're this intelligent organism.
I mean, does that give you pause how incredible it is that the evolutionary dynamics
that you're describing is actually happening and they're figuring out how to jump from bats to humans,
all in this distributed fashion.
And then most of us don't even say they're alive or intelligent or whatever.
So intelligence is in the eye of the beholder.
Stupid is a stupid does, as Forrest Gump would say.
And intelligence is as intelligent does.
So basically, if the virus is finding solutions that we think of as intelligent,
yeah, it's probably intelligent.
But that's again in the eye of the beholder.
Do you think viruses are intelligent?
Of course not.
Really?
No.
It's so incredible.
So remember when I was talking about the two components of evolution.
One is the stupid mutation, which is completely blind.
And the other one is the super smart selection, which is ruthless.
So it's not viruses who are smart.
It's this component of evolution that's smart.
So it's evolution that sort of appears smart.
And how is that happening by huge parallel search across thousands of, you know,
parallel infections throughout the world right now?
Yes, but so to put back on that.
So yes, so then the intelligence is in the mechanism.
But then by that argument, viruses would be more intelligent because there's just more of them.
So the search, they're basically the brute force search that's happening with viruses
because there's so many more of them than humans,
then they're taken as a whole or more intelligent.
I mean, so you don't think it's possible that, I mean, who runs,
would we even be here if viruses weren't, I mean, who runs this thing?
So let me answer your question.
So we would not be here if it wasn't for viruses.
And part of the reason is that if you look at mammalian evolution early on
in this mammalian radiation that basically happened after the death of the dinosaurs
is that some of the viruses that we had in our genome
spread throughout our genome and created binding sites for new classes of regulatory proteins.
And these binding sites that landed all over our genome are now control elements
that basically control our genes and sort of help the complexity of the circuitry of mammalian genomes.
So, you know, everything's co-evolution.
We're working together.
We've co-opted them.
No, I never said they're dumb.
They just don't care.
They don't care.
Another thing, oh, is the virus trying to kill us?
No, it's not.
The virus is not trying to kill you.
It's actually actively trying to not kill you.
So when you get infected, if you die, bummer, I killed him.
It's what the reaction of the virus will be.
Why?
Because that virus won't spread.
Many people have a misconception of viruses are smart or viruses are mean.
They don't care.
It's like you have to clean yourself of any kind of anthropomorphism out there.
I don't know.
Oh, yes.
So there's a sense when taken as a whole that there's...
Tim, you have to be holder.
Stupid is a stupid does.
Intelligent is a stupid.
Intelligent does.
So if you want to call them intelligent, that's fine.
Because the end result is that they're finding amazing solutions.
Right.
I mean, oh.
But they're so dumb about it.
They're just doing dumb.
They don't care.
They don't care.
They don't care.
The care word is really interesting.
Exactly.
I mean, there could be an argument that they're conscious.
They're just dividing.
They're not.
They're just dividing.
They're just a little entity which happens to be dividing and spreading.
It doesn't want to kill us.
In fact, it prefers not to kill us.
It just wants to spread.
And when I say once, again, I'm anthropomorphizing, but it's just that if you have two versions
of a virus, one acquires a mutation that spreads more, that's going to spread more.
One acquires a mutation that spreads less, that's going to be lost.
One acquires a mutation that enters faster, that's going to be kept.
One acquires a mutation that kills you right away, it's going to be lost.
So over evolutionary time, the virus is that spread super well, but don't kill the host
or the ones that are going to survive.
Yeah, but so you brilliantly described the basic mechanisms of how it all happens, but
when you zoom out and you see the, you know, the entirety of viruses, maybe across different
strains of viruses, it seems like a living organism.
I am in awe of biology.
I find biology amazingly beautiful.
I find the design of the current coronavirus, however lethal it is, amazingly beautiful.
The way that it is encoded, the way that it tricks your cells into making 30 proteins
from a single RNA.
Human cells don't do that.
Human cells make one protein from each RNA molecule.
They don't make two, they make one.
We are hardwired to make only one protein from every RNA molecule.
And yet this virus goes in, throws in a single messenger RNA.
Just like any messenger RNA, we have tens of thousands of messenger RNAs in our cells
in any one time, in every one of our cells.
It throws in one RNA and that RNA is so, I'm going to use your word here, not my word,
intelligent, that it hijacks the entire machinery of your human cell.
It basically has at the beginning a giant open reading frame.
That's a giant protein that gets translated.
Two thirds of that RNA make a single giant protein.
That single protein is basically what a human cell would make.
It's like, oh, here's a start code.
I'm going to start translating here.
Human cells are kind of dumb.
I'm sorry.
Again, this is not the word that we'd normally use, but the human cell basically says, oh,
this is an RNA.
It must be mine.
Let me translate.
And it starts translating it.
And then you're in trouble.
Why?
Because that one protein, as it's growing, gets cleaved into about 20 different peptides.
The first peptide and the second peptide start interacting and the third one and the fourth
one.
And they shut off the ribosome of the whole cell to not translate human RNAs anymore.
So the virus basically hijacks your cells and it cuts, it cleaves every one of your
human RNAs to basically say to the ribosome, don't translate this one junk.
Don't look at this one junk.
And it only spares its own RNAs because they have a particular mark that it spares.
Then all of the ribosomes that normally make protein in your human cells are now only able
to translate viral RNAs.
They're more and more and more and more of them.
That's the first 20 proteins.
In fact, halfway down about protein 11, between 11 and 12, you basically have a translational
slippage where the ribosome skips reading frame.
And it translates from one reading frame to another reading frame.
That means that about half of them are going to be translated from one to 11.
And the other half are going to be translated from 12 to 16.
It's gorgeous.
And then you're done.
Then that mRNA will never translate the last 10 proteins, but spike is the one right after
that one.
So how does spike even get translated?
This positive strand RNA virus has a reverse transcriptase, which is an RNA based reverse
transcriptase.
So from the RNA on the positive strand, it makes an RNA on the negative strand.
And in between every single one of these genes, these open reading frames, there's a little
signal, AACGCA or something like that, that basically loops over to the beginning of the
RNA.
And basically instead of sort of having a single full negative strand RNA, it basically has
a partial negative strand RNA that ends right before the beginning of that gene.
And another one that ends right before the beginning of that gene.
These negative strand RNAs now make positive strand RNAs that then look to the human host
cell, just like any other human mRNA.
It's like, oh, great.
I'm going to translate that one because it doesn't have the cleaving that the virus has
now put on all your human genes.
And then you've lost the battle.
That cell is now only making proteins for the virus that will then create the spike protein,
the envelope protein, the membrane protein, the nucleocapsid protein that will package
up the RNA and then sort of create new viral envelopes.
And these will then be secreted out of that cell in new little packages that will then
infect the rest of the cells.
Repeat the whole process again.
It's beautiful, right?
It's mind-blowing.
It's hard not to anthropomorphize it.
I know, but it's so gorgeous.
So there is a beauty to it.
Of course.
Is it terrifying to you?
So this is something that has happened throughout history.
Humans have been nearly wiped out over and over and over again, and yet never fully wiped
out.
So I'm not concerned about the human race.
I'm not even concerned about the impact on sort of our survival as a species.
This is absolutely something, I mean, human life is so invaluable, and every one of us
is so invaluable.
But if you think of it as sort of, is this the end of our species?
By no means, basically.
So let me explain.
The Black Death killed what, 30% of Europe?
That has left a tremendous imprint, a huge hole, a horrendous hole in the genetic makeup
of humans.
There's been series of wiping out of huge fractions of entire species or just entire
species altogether, and that has a consequence on the human immune repertoire.
If you look at how Europe was shaped and how Africa was shaped by malaria, for example,
all the individuals that carry a mutation that protects you from malaria were able to
survive much more.
And if you look at the frequency of sickle cell disease and the frequency of malaria,
the maps are actually showing the same pattern, the same imprint on Africa.
And that basically led people to hypothesize that the reason why sickle cell disease is
so much more frequent in Americans of African descent is because there was selection in
Africa against malaria, leading to sickle cell, because when the cell sickle, malaria
is not able to replicate inside your cells as well, and therefore you protect against
that.
So if you look at human disease, all of the genetic associations that we do with human
disease, you basically see the imprint of these waves of selection killing off gazillions
of humans.
And there's so many immune processes that are coming up as associated with so many different
diseases.
The reason for that is similar to what I was describing earlier, where the outward-facing
proteins evolve much more rapidly because the environment is always changing.
But what's really interesting in the human genome is that we have co-opted many of these
immune genes to carry out non-immune functions.
For example, in our brain, we use immune cells to cleave off neuronal connections that don't
get used.
This whole use it or lose it, we know the mechanism.
It's microglia that cleave off neuronal synaptic connections that are just not utilized.
When you utilize them, you mark them in a particular way that basically when the microglia
come, tell it, don't kill this one, it's used now.
And the microglia will go off and kill it once you don't use.
This is an immune function, which is co-opted to do non-immune things.
If you look at our adipocytes, M1 versus M2 macrophages inside our fat will basically
determine whether you're obese or not.
And these are, again, immune cells that are resident and living within these tissues.
So many disease associations...
That we co-opt these kinds of things for incredibly complicated functions.
Exactly.
Evolution works in so many different ways, which are all beautiful and mysterious.
But not intelligent.
Not intelligent.
It's in the eye of the beholder.
But the point that I'm trying to make is that if you look at the imprint that COVID will
have, hopefully it will not be big.
Hopefully the US will get attacked together and stop the virus from spreading further.
But if it doesn't, it's having an imprint on individuals who have particular genetic
repertoires.
So if you look at now the genetic associations of blood type and immune function cells,
et cetera, there's actually association, genetic variation that basically says how
much more likely am I or you to die if we contact the virus.
And it's through these rounds of shaping the human genome that humans have basically
made it so far.
And selection is ruthless and it's brutal and it only comes with a lot of killing.
But this is the way that viruses and environments have shaped the human genome.
Basically, when you go through periods of famine, you select for particular genes.
And what's left is not necessarily better, it's just whatever survived.
And it may have been the surviving one back then, not because it was better, maybe the
ones that ran slower survived.
I mean, you know, again, not necessarily better.
But the surviving ones are basically the ones that then are shaped for any kind of subsequent
evolutionary condition and environmental condition.
But if you look at, for example, obesity, obesity was selected for basically the genes
that now predisposes to obesity were at 2% frequency in Africa.
They rose to 44% frequency in Europe because you basically went through the ice ages and
there was a scarcity of food, so, you know, there was a selection to being able to store
every single calorie you consume.
Eventually environment changes.
So the better allele, which was the fat storing allele, became the worse allele because it's
the fat storing allele.
It still has the same function.
So if you look at my genome, speaking of mom calling, mom gave me a bad copy of that gene,
this FTO locus.
Basically, what it has to do with obesity, yeah, basically now have a bad copy from mom
that makes me more likely to be obese.
And I also have a bad copy from dad that makes me more likely to be obese.
I'm homozygous.
And that's the allele.
It's still the minor allele, but it's at 44% frequency in Southeast Asia, 42% frequency
in Europe, even though it started at 2%.
It was an awesome allele to have 100 years ago.
Right now, it's a pretty terrible allele.
So the other concept is that diversity matters.
If we had 100 million nuclear physicists living the earth right now, we'd be in trouble.
You need diversity.
You need artists, and you need musicians, and you need mathematicians, and you need,
you know, politicians.
Yes, even those.
And you need like...
Well, let's not get crazy.
Because then if a virus comes along or whatever...
Exactly.
Exactly.
There are two reasons.
Number one, you want diversity in the immune repertoire, and we have built-in diversity.
So basically, there are the most diverse...
Basically if you look at our immune system, there's layers and layers of diversity.
Like the way that you create your cells generates diversity because of the selection for the
VDGA recombination that basically eventually leads to a huge number of repertoires.
But that's only one small component of diversity.
The blood type is another one, the major histocopatibility complex, the HLA alleles are, you know, another
source of diversity.
So the immune system of humans is by nature incredibly diverse, and that basically leads
to resilience.
So basically what I'm saying is that I don't worry for the human species because we are
so diverse immunologically, we are likely to be very resilient against so many different
attacks like this current virus.
So you're saying natural pandemics may not be something that you're really afraid of
because of the diversity in our genetic makeup.
What about engineered pandemics?
Do you have fears of us messing with the makeup of viruses or...
Well, yeah, let's say with the makeup of viruses to create something that we can't control
and would be much more destructive than it would come about naturally.
Remember how we were talking about how smart evolution is?
Humans are much dumber.
You mean like human scientists, engineers?
Yeah, humans.
Humans just like...
Humans overall.
Yeah, humans overall.
But I mean, even the sort of synthetic biologists, basically, if you were to create virus like
SARS that will kill a lot of people, you would probably start with SARS.
So whoever would like to design such a thing would basically start with SARS tree or at
least some relative of SARS.
The source genome for the current virus was something completely different.
It was something that has never infected humans.
No one in their right mind would have started there.
But when you say source, it's like the nearest...
The nearest relative is in a whole other branch, no species of which has ever infected humans
in that branch.
So let's put this to rest.
This was not designed by someone to kill off the human race.
You don't believe it was engineered?
The...
Well, likely.
Yeah, the path to engineering a deadly virus would not come from this strain that was used.
Moreover, there's been various claims of, haha, this was mixed and matched in the lab
because the S1 protein has three different components, each of which has a different evolutionary
tree.
So, you know, a lot of popular press basically said, aha, this came from pangolin and this
came from, you know, all kinds of other species.
This is what has been happening throughout the coronavirus tree.
So basically, the S1 protein has been recombining across species all the time.
Remember when I was talking about the positive strand, the negative strand, subgenomic RNAs,
these can actually recombine and if you have two different viruses infecting the same cell,
they can actually mix and match between the positive strand and the negative strand and
basically create a new hybrid virus with recombination that now has the S1 from one and the rest
of the genome from another.
And this is something that happens a lot in S1, in Orphe, et cetera.
And that's something that's true of the whole tree.
For the whole family of known viruses.
Exactly.
So it's not like someone has been messing with this for millions of years and, you know,
changing the situation.
So it happens naturally.
That's, again, beautiful that that somehow happens, that they recombine.
So two different strands can infect the body and then recombine.
So all of this actually magic happens inside hosts.
Like all like...
Yeah.
That's why classification-wise, virus is not thought to be alive because it doesn't self-replicate.
It's not autonomous.
It's something that enters a living cell and then co-ops it to basically make it its own.
But by itself, people ask me, how do we kill this bastard?
I'm like, you stop it from replicating.
It's not like a bacterium that will just live in a, you know, puddle or something.
It's a virus.
Viruses don't live without their hosts.
And they only live within their hosts for very little time.
So if you stop it from replicating, it'll stop from spreading.
I mean, it's not like HIV, which can stay dormant for a long time.
Basically, coronavirus is just don't do that.
They're not integrating genomes.
They're RNA genomes.
So if it's not expressed, it degrades.
RNA degrades.
It doesn't just stick around.
Well, let me ask also about the immune system you mentioned.
A lot of people kind of ask, you know, how can we strengthen the immune system to respond
to this particular virus, when the virus is in general?
Do you have from a biological perspective thoughts on what we can do as humans to strengthen
our immune system?
If you look at the death rates across different countries, people with less vaccination have
been dying more.
If you look at North Italy, the vaccination rates are abysmal there, and a lot of people
have been dying.
If you look at Greece, very good vaccination rates, almost no one has been dying.
So yes, there's a policy component.
So Italy reacted very slowly.
Greece reacted very fast.
So yeah, many fewer people died in Greece.
But there might actually be a component of a genetic immune repertoire, basically how
did people die off, you know, in the history of the Greek population versus the Italian
population.
Wow.
That's interesting to think about.
And then there's a component of what vaccinations did you have as a kid and what are the off-target
effects of those vaccinations?
So basically, a vaccination can have two components.
One is training your immune system against that specific insult.
The second one is boosting up your immune system for all kinds of other things.
If you look at allergies, Northern Europe, super clean environments, tons of allergies.
Southern Europe, my kids grew up eating dirt.
No allergies.
So growing up, I never had even heard of what allergies are, like really allergies.
And the reason is that I was playing in the garden.
I was putting all kinds of stuff in my mouth from, you know, all kinds of dirt and stuff,
tons of viruses, there are tons of bacteria there.
You know, my immune system was built up.
So the more you protect your immune system from exposure, the less opportunity it has
to learn about non-self repertoire in a way that prepares it for the next insult.
So it's a horizontal thing too, like the, it's just throughout your lifetime and the
lifetime of the people that, your ancestors, that kind of thing.
Yeah.
Absolutely.
And it returns against free will on the free will side of things.
Is there something we could do to strengthen our immune system in 2020?
Is there like, you know, exercise, diet, all that kind of stuff.
So it's kind of funny.
There's a cartoon that basically shows two windows with a teller in each window.
One has a humongous line and the other one has no one.
The one that has no one above says health, no, it says exercise and diet.
And the other one says pill and there's a huge line for pill.
So we're looking basically for magic bullets for sort of ways that we can, you know, beat
cancer and beat coronavirus and beat this and beat that.
And it turns out that the window with like just diet and exercise is the best way to
boost every aspect of your health.
If you look at Alzheimer's, exercise and nutrition, I mean, you're like, really?
For my brain neurodegeneration?
Absolutely.
If you look at cancer, exercise and nutrition, if you look at coronavirus, exercise and nutrition,
every single aspect of human health gets improved.
And one of the studies we're doing now is basically looking at what are the effects
of diet and exercise?
How similar are they to each other?
We basically take in diet intervention and exercise intervention in human and in mice.
And we're basically doing single cell profiling of a bunch of different tissues to basically
understand how are the cells, both the stromal cells and the immune cells of each of these
tissues responding to the effect of exercise?
What are the communication networks between different cells where the muscle that exercises
sends signals through the bloodstream, through the lymphatic system, through all kinds of
other systems that give signals to other cells that I have exercised and you should change
in this particular way, which basically reconfigure those receptor cells with the effect of exercise?
How well understood is those reconfigurations?
Very little.
We're just starting now, basically.
Is the hope there to understand the effect on the immune system?
The effect on brain, the effect on your liver, on your digestive system, on your adipocytes?
Adipose, you know, the most misunderstood organ.
Everybody thinks, oh, fat, terrible.
No, fat is awesome.
Your fat cells is what's keeping you alive.
Because if you didn't have your fat cells, all those lipids and all those calories would
be floating around in your blood and you'd be dead by now.
Your adipocytes are your best friend.
They're basically storing all these excess calories so that they don't hurt all of the
rest of the body.
And they're also fat-burning in many ways.
So, you know, again, when you don't have the homozygous version that I have, your cells
are able to burn calories much more easily by sort of flipping a master metabolic switch
that involves these FTO locus that I mentioned earlier and its target genes, IRIX3 and RX5,
that basically switch your adipocytes during their three first days of differentiation
as they're becoming mature adipocytes to basically become either fat-burning or fat-storing fat
cells.
And the fat-burning fat cells are your best friends.
They're much closer to muscle than they are to white adipocytes.
Is there a lot of difference between people like that you could give, science could eventually
give advice that is very generalizable?
Or is our differences in our genetic makeup, like you mentioned, is that going to be basically
something we have to be very specialized to individuals?
Any advice we give on terms of diet, like what we're just talking about?
Believe it or not, the most personalized advice that you give for nutrition don't have to
do with your genome.
They have to do with your gut microbiome, with the bacteria that live inside you.
So most of your digestion is actually happening by species that are not human inside you.
You have more non-human cells than you have human cells.
You're basically a giant bag of bacteria with a few human cells along.
And those do not necessarily have to do with your genetic makeup?
They interact with your genetic makeup.
They interact with your epigenome.
They interact with your nutrition.
They interact with your environment.
They're basically an additional source of variation.
So when you're thinking about personalized nutritional advice, part of that is actually
how do you match your microbiome?
And part of that is how do we match your genetics?
But again, this is a very diverse set of contributors.
And the effect sizes are not enormous, so I think the science for that is not fully developed
yet.
Speaking of diets, because I've wrestled in combat sports, but sports my whole life
were weight matters, so you have to cut and all that stuff.
One thing I've learned a lot about my body, and it seems to be, I think, true about other
people's bodies is that you can adjust to a lot of things.
That's the miraculous thing about this biological system is I fast often.
I used to eat five, six times a day and thought that was absolutely necessary.
How could you not eat that often?
And then when I started fasting, your body adjusted that and you learned how to not
eat.
And if you just give it a chance for a few weeks, actually, over a period of a few weeks,
your body can adjust to anything.
And that's a miracle.
That's such a beautiful thing.
So I'm a computer scientist, and I've basically gone through periods of 24 hours without eating
or stopping, and then I'm like, oh, must eat, and I eat a ton.
I used to order two pizzas just with my brother.
So I've gone through these extremes as well, and I've gone the whole intermittent fasting
thing.
So I can sympathize with you both on the seven meals a day to the zero meals a day.
So I think when I say everything with moderation, I actually think your body responds interestingly
to these different changes in diet.
I think part of the reason why we lose weight with pretty much every kind of change in behavior
is because our epigenome and the set of proteins and enzymes that are expressed and our microbiome
are not well suited to that nutritional source.
And therefore, they will not be able to sort of catch everything that you give them.
And then a lot of that will go undigested.
And that basically means that your body can then lose weight in the short term, but very
quickly will adjust to that new normal, and then we'll be able to sort of perhaps gain
a lot of weight from the diet.
So anyway, I mean, there's also studies in factories where basically people dim the
lights, and then suddenly everybody started working better.
It was like, wow, that's amazing.
Three weeks later, they made the lights a little brighter.
Everybody started working better.
So any kind of intervention has a placebo effect of, wow, now I'm healthier and I'm
going to be running more often, et cetera.
So it's very hard to uncouple the placebo effect of, wow, I'm doing something to intervene
on my diet from the, wow, this is actually the right thing for me.
So, you know.
Yeah.
From the perspective from nutrition science, psychology, both things I'm interested in,
especially psychology, it seems that it's extremely difficult to do good science because
there's so many variables involved.
It's so difficult to control the variables.
So difficult to do sufficiently large scale experiments, both sort of in terms of number
of subjects and temporal, like how long you do the study for, that it just seems like
it's not even a real science for now, like nutrition science.
I want to jump into the whole placebo effect for a little bit here and basically talk about
the implications of that.
If I give you a sugar pill and I tell you it's a sugar pill, you won't get any better.
But if I tell you a sugar pill and I tell you, wow, this is an amazing drug, it actually
will stop your cancer, your cancer will actually stop with much higher probability.
What does that mean?
That's so amazing.
That means that if I can trick your brain into thinking that I'm healing you, your brain
will basically figure out a way to heal itself, to heal the body.
And that tells us that there's so much that we don't understand in the interplay between
our cognition and our biology, that if we were able to better harvest the power of our
brain to sort of impact the body through the placebo effect, we would be so much better
in so many different things.
Just by tricking yourself into thinking that you're doing better, you're actually doing
better.
So there's something to be said about sort of positive thinking, about optimism, about
sort of just getting your brain and your mind into the right mindset that helps your body
and helps your entire biology.
From a science perspective, that's just fascinating.
Obviously, most things about the brain is a total mystery for now, but that's a fascinating
interplay that the brain can reduce.
The brain can help cure cancer, I don't even know what to do with that.
I mean, the way to think about that is the following.
The converse of the equation is something that we are much more comfortable with.
Like, oh, if you're stressed, then your heart might rise and all kinds of sort of toxins
might be released and that can have a detrimental effect on your body, et cetera, et cetera.
So maybe it's easier to understand your body healing from your mind by your mind is not
killing your body, or at least it's killing it less.
So I think that aspect of the stress equation is a little easier for most of us to conceptualize,
but then the healing part is perhaps the same pathways, perhaps different pathways, but
again, something that is totally untapped scientifically.
I think we try to bring this question up a couple of times, but let's return to it again
as what do you think is the difference between the way a computer represents information,
the human genome represents and stores information?
And maybe broadly, what is the difference between how you think about computers and how
you think about biological systems?
So I made a very provocative claim earlier that we are a digital computer, like that
at the core lies a digital code.
And that's true in many ways, but surrounding that digital core, there's a huge amount of
analog.
So if you look at our brain, it's not really digital, if you look at our sort of RNA and
all of that stuff inside ourselves, not really digital, it's really analog in many ways.
But let's start with the code and then we'll expand to the rest.
So the code itself is digital.
So there's genes, you can think of the genes as, I don't know, the procedures, the functions
inside your language.
And then somehow you have to turn these functions on.
How do you call the gene?
How do you call that function?
The way that you would do it in old programming languages is go to address whatever in your
memory and then you'd start running from there.
And modern programming languages have encapsulated this into functions and objects and all of
that.
And it's nice and cute, but in the end deep down, there's still an assembly code that
says go to that instruction and it runs that instruction.
If you look at the human genome and the genome of pretty much most species out there, there's
no go to function.
You just don't start in transcribing in position 1300, 13,527 in chromosome 12.
You instead have content-based indexing.
So at every location in the genome, in front of the genes that need to be turned on, I
don't know when you drink coffee, there's a little coffee marker in front of all of
them.
And whenever your cells that metabolize coffee need to metabolize coffee, they basically see
coffee and they're like, ooh, let's go turn on all the coffee-marked genes.
So there's basically these small motifs, these small sequences that we call regulatory motifs.
They're like patterns of DNA.
They're only eight characters long or so, like GAT, GCA, et cetera.
And these motifs work in combinations and every one of them has some recruitment affinity
for a different protein that will then come and bind it.
And together, collections of these motifs create regions that we call regulatory regions
that can be either promoters near the beginning of the gene, and that basically tells you
where the function actually starts, where you call it, and then enhancers that are looping
around of the DNA that basically bring the machinery that binds those enhancers and then
bring it onto the promoter, which then recruits the right sort of the ribosome and the polymerase
and all of that thing, which will first transcribe and then export and then eventually translate
in the cytoplasm, whatever RNA molecule.
So the beauty of the way that the digital computer that's the genome works is that it's
extremely fault tolerant.
If I took your hard drive and I messed with 20% of the letters in it, of those zeroes
and ones and I flipped them, you'd be in trouble.
If I take the genome and I flipped 20% of the letters, you probably won't even notice.
And that resilience is a key design principle, and again, I'm thrombomorphizing here, but
it's a key driving principle of how biological systems work.
They're first resilient and then anything else.
And when you look at these incredible beauty of life from the most, I don't know, beautiful,
I don't know, human genome maybe of humanity and all of the ideals that come with it to
the most terrifying genome like, I don't know, COVID-19, SARS-CoV-2 and the current pandemic,
you basically see this elegance as the epitome of clean design, but it's dirty.
It's a mess.
It's, you know, the way to get there is hugely messy.
And that's something that we as computer scientists don't embrace.
We like to have clean code, you know, as like in engineering, they teach you about compartmentalization,
about sort of separating functions, about modularity, about hierarchical design, none
of that applies in biology.
Testing.
Testing, sure.
Yeah, biology does plenty of that, but I mean, through evolutionary exploration.
But if you look at biological systems, first they are robust and then they specialize to
become anything else.
And if you look at viruses, the reason why they're so elegant when you look at the design
of this, you know, genome, it seems so elegant.
And the reason for that is that it's been stripped down from something much larger because
of the pressure to keep it compact.
So many compact genomes out there have ancestors that were much larger.
You don't start small and become big.
You go through a loop of add a bunch of stuff, increase complexity, and then, you know, slim
it down.
And one of my early papers was, in fact, on genome duplication.
One of the things we found is that baker's yeast, which is the yeast that you use to
make bread, but also the yeast that you use to make wine, which is basically the dominant
species when you go in the fields of Tuscany and you say, you know, what's out there?
It's basically Saccharomyces cerevisiae.
Or the way my Italian friends say, Saccharomyces cerevisiae.
Which means what?
Oh, Saccharomyces.
Okay.
I'm sorry.
I'm Greek.
So, yeah.
Zaharo.
Zaharo is sugar.
Micis is fungus.
Yes.
Cerevisiae.
Cerveza.
Beer.
So it means the sugar fungus of beer.
Yeah.
You know, let's, let's, let's be sounding to the you.
Still poetic.
Yeah.
Anyway, Saccharomyces cerevisiae, basically the major baker's yeast out there is the descendant
of a whole gene duplication.
Why would a whole gene duplication even happen?
When it happened is coinciding with about a hundred million years ago and the emergence
of fruit bearing plants.
Why fruit bearing plants?
Because animals would eat the fruit and would walk around and poop huge amounts of nutrients
along with a seed for the plants to spread.
Before that, plants were not spreading through animals.
They were spreading through wind and all kinds of other ways.
But basically the moment you have fruit bearing plants, the, the, the, these plants are basically
creating this abundance of sugar in the environment.
So there's an evolutionary niche that gets created.
And in that evolutionary niche, you basically have enough sugar that a whole gene duplication
which initially is a very messy event allows you to then, you know, relieve some of that
complexity.
So to pause, what does genome duplication mean?
That basically means that instead of having eight chromosomes, you're gonna have 16 chromosomes.
So, but the duplication at first, when you have six, when you go to 16, you're not using
that.
Oh yeah, you are.
Yeah.
So when you go to the next, you went from having eight chromosomes to having 16 chromosomes,
probably a non-disjunction event during a duplication, during a division.
So you basically divide the cell, instead of half the genome going this way and half
the genome going the other way, after duplication of the genome, you basically have all of it
going to one cell.
And then there's a sufficient messiness there that you end up with slight differences that
make most of these chromosomes be actually preserved.
It's a long story short to basically-
But that's a big upgrade, right?
So that's-
Not necessarily, because what happens immediately thereafter is that you start massively losing
tons of those duplicated genes.
So 90% of those genes were actually lost very rapidly after whole genome duplication.
And the reason for that is that biology is not intelligent, it's just ruthless selection,
random mutation.
So the ruthless selection basically means that as soon as one of the random mutations
hit one gene, ruthless selection just kills off that gene.
It's just-
If you have a pressure to maintain a small compact genome, you will very rapidly lose
the second copy of most of your genes, and a small number, 10%, were kept into copies.
And those had to do a lot with environment adaptation, with the speed of replication,
with the speed of translation, and with trigger processing.
So I'm making a long story short to basically say that evolution is messy.
The only way- like so the example that I was giving of messing with 20% of your bits in
your computer, totally bogus, duplicating all your functions and just throwing them
out there in the same function, just totally bogus, like this would never work in an engineer
system.
But biological systems, because of this content-based indexing and because of this modularity that
comes from the fact that the gene is controlled by a series of tags, and now if you need
this gene in another setting, you just add some more tags that will basically turn it
on also in those settings.
So this gene is now pressured to do two different functions, and it builds up complexity.
I see a whole gene duplication and gene duplication in general as a way to relieve that complexity.
So you have this gradual buildup of complexity as functions get sort of added onto the existing
genes.
And boom, you duplicate your workforce, and you now have two copies of this gene.
One will probably specialize to do one, and the other one will specialize to do the other,
or one will maintain the ancestral function, the other one will sort of be free to evolve
and specialize while losing the ancestral function and so on and so forth.
So that's how genomes evolve.
They're just messy things, but they're extremely fault tolerant, and they're extremely able
to deal with mutations because that's the very way that you generate new functions.
So new functionalization comes from the very thing that breaks it.
So even in the current pandemic, many people are asking me which mutations matter the most.
And what I tell them is, well, we can study the evolutionary dynamics of the current genome
to then understand which mutations have previously happened or not, and which mutations happen
in genes that evolve rapidly or not.
And one of the things we found, for example, is that the genes that evolved rapidly in
the past are still evolving rapidly now in the current pandemic.
The genes that evolved slowly in the past are still evolving slowly.
Which means that they're useful.
Which means that they're under the same evolutionary pressures, but then the question is what happens
in specific mutations.
So if you look at the D614 gene mutation that's been all over the news, so in position
D614, in the amino acids D614 of the S protein, there's a D2G mutation that sort of has crept
over the population.
That mutation we found out through my work disrupts a perfectly conserved nucleotide
position that has never been changed in the history of millions of years of equivalent
mammalian evolution of these viruses.
That basically means that it's a completely new adaptation to human.
And that mutation has now gone from 1% frequency to 90% frequency in almost all outbreaks.
So there's a mutation, I like how you said in the mute, the 416, what was it?
Yeah, 614, sorry.
614.
D614G.
D614G.
So literally, so what you're saying is this is like a chest move.
So it just mutated one letter to another.
Exactly.
It didn't happen before.
And this somehow, this mutation is really useful.
It's really useful in the current environment of the genome, which is moving from human
to human.
When it was moving from bat to bat, it couldn't care less for that mutation, but it's environment
specific.
So now that it's moving from human to human, it's moving way better, like by orders of
magnitude.
Okay, so you're like tracking this evolutionary dynamics, which is fascinating.
But what do you do with that?
So what does that mean?
What do you make of this mutation in trying to anticipate, I guess, is one of the things
you're trying to do is anticipate where, how this unrolls into the future, this evolutionary
dynamics.
Such a great question.
So there's two things.
Remember when I was saying earlier, mutation is the path to new things, but also the path
to break old things.
So what we know is that this position was extremely preserved through gazillions of mutations.
That mutation was never tolerated when it was moving from bats to bats.
So that basically means that that position is extremely important in the function of
that protein.
That's the first thing it tells.
The second one is that that position was very well suited to bat transmission, but now is
not well suited to human transmission, so it got rid of it.
And it now has a new version of that amino acid that basically makes it much easier to
transmit from human to human.
So in terms of the evolutionary history teaching us about the future, it basically tells us
here's the regions that are currently mutating.
Here's the regions that are most likely to mutate going forward.
As you're building a vaccine, here's what you should be focusing on in terms of the
most stable regions that are the least likely to mutate.
Or here's the newly evolved functions that are most likely to be important because they've
overcome this local maximum that it had reached in the bat transmission.
So anyway, it's a tangent to basically say that evolution works in messy ways.
And the thing that you would break is the thing that actually allows you to first go
through a lull and then reach a new local maximum.
And I often like to say that if engineers had basically designed evolution, we would
still be perfectly replicating bacteria because it's by making the bacterium worse that you
allow evolution to reach a new optimum.
That's just a pause on that, that's so profound for the entirety of this scientific and engineering
disciplines.
Exactly.
We as engineers need to embrace breaking things.
We as engineers need to embrace robustness as the first principle beyond perfection because
nothing's going to ever be perfect.
And when you're sending a satellite to Mars, when something goes wrong, it'll break down
as opposed to building systems that tolerate failure and are resilient to that and in fact
get better through that.
So the SpaceX approach versus NASA for the...
For example.
Is there something we can learn about the incredible, take lessons from the incredible
biological systems in their resilience in their in the mushiness, the messiness to our
computing systems, to our computers?
It would basically be starting from scratch in many ways.
It would basically be building new paradigms that don't try to get the right answer all
the time, but try to get the right answer most of the time or a lot of the time.
Do you see deep learning systems in the whole world of machine learning as kind of taking
a step in that direction?
Absolutely.
Basically, by allowing this much more natural evolution of these parameters, you basically...
And if you look at sort of deep learning systems, again, they're not inspired by the genome aspect
of biology.
They're inspired by the brain aspect of biology.
And again, I want you to pause for a second and realize the complexity of the entire human
brain with trillions of connections within our neurons, with millions of cells talking
to each other, is still encoded within that same genome, that same genome encodes every
single freaking cell type of the entire body.
Every single cell is encoded by the same code, and yet specialization allows you to have
the single viral-like genome that self-replicates, the single module, modular automaton, work
with other copies of itself, it's mind-boggling.
Create complex organs through which blood flows.
And what is that blood?
The same freaking genome.
Create organs that communicate with each other.
And what are these organs?
The exact same genome.
Create a brain that is innervated by massive amounts of blood pumping energy to it, 20%
of our energetic needs, to the brain from the same genome.
And all of the neuronal connections, all of the auxiliary cells, all of the immune cells,
the astrocytes, the ligature cells, the neurons, the excitatory, the inhibitory neurons, all
of the different classes of parasites, the blood-brain barrier, all of that, same genome.
One way to see that in a sad, this one is beautiful, the sad thing is thinking about
the trillions of organisms that died to create that.
You mean on the evolutionary path?
Yeah, on the evolutionary path to humans.
It's crazy.
There's two dissenters of apes just talking on a podcast, okay, it's just so mind-boggling.
Just to boggle our minds a little bit more.
Just talking to each other.
We are basically generating a series of vocal utterances through our pulsating of vocal
cords received through this.
The people who listen to this are taking a completely different path to that information
transfer yet through language.
But imagine if we could connect these brains directly to each other.
The amount of information that I'm condensing into a small number of words is a huge funnel
which then you receive and you expand into a huge number of thoughts from that small
funnel.
In many ways, engineers would love to have the whole information transfer, just take
the whole set of neurons and throw them away, I mean throw them to the other person.
This might actually not be better because in your misinterpretation of every word that
I'm saying, you are creating new interpretation that might actually be way better than what
I meant in the first place.
The ambiguity of language perhaps might be the secret to creativity.
Every single time you work on a project by yourself, you only bounce ideas with one person
and your neurons are basically fully cognizant of what these ideas are.
But the moment you interact with another person, the misinterpretations that happen might be
the most creative part of the process.
With my students, every time we have a research meeting, I very often pause and say, let me
repeat what you just said in a different way.
And I sort of go on and brainstorm with what they were saying, but by the third time, it's
not what they were saying at all.
And when they pick up what I'm saying, they're like, oh, well, they've sort of learned something
very different from what I was saying.
And that is the same kind of messiness that I'm describing in the genome itself.
It's sort of embracing the messiness.
And that's a feature, not a book.
Exactly.
And in the same way, when you're thinking about sort of these deep learning systems that
will allow us to sort of be more creative perhaps or learn better approximations of
these complex functions, again, tuned to the universe that we inhabit, you have to embrace
the breaking.
You have to embrace the, you know, how do we get out of these local optima?
And a lot of the design paradigms that have made deep learning so successful are ways
to get away from that, ways to get better training by sort of sending long range messages,
these LSTM models and the, you know, sort of feed forward loops that, you know, sort
of jump through layers of a convolutional neural network.
All of these things are basically ways to push you out of these local maxima.
And that's sort of what evolution does.
That's what language does.
That's what conversation and brainstorming does.
That's what our brain does.
So, you know, this design paradigm is something that's pervasive and yet not taught in schools,
not taught in engineering schools, where everything's minutely modularized to make
sure that we never deviate from, you know, whatever signal we're trying to emit, as opposed
to let all hell breaks loose because that's the, that's the path to paradise, the path
to paradise.
Yeah.
I mean, it's difficult to know how to teach that and what to do with it.
I mean, it's, it's difficult to know how to build up a sign, the scientific method around
messiness.
I mean, it's not all messiness.
We need, we need some cleanness and going back to the example with Mars, that's probably
the place where I want to sort of moderate error as much as possible and sort of control
the environment as much as possible.
But if you're trying to repopulate Mars, well, maybe messiness is a good thing then.
On that, you quickly mentioned this in terms of us using our vocal chords to speak on a
podcast, so Elon Musk and Neuralink are working on trying to plug as per our discussion with
computers and biological systems to connect it to, he's trying to connect our brain to
a computer to create a brain computer interface where they can communicate back and forth.
On this line of thinking, do you think this is possible to bridge the gap between our engineered
computing systems and the messy biological systems?
My answer would be absolutely.
We, we, you know, there's no doubt that we can understand more and more about what goes
on in the brain and we can sort of train the brain.
I don't know if you remember the Palm Pilot.
Yeah.
Palm Pilot, yeah.
Do you remember this whole sort of alphabet that they had created?
Am I thinking of the same thing?
It's basically you had, you had a little pen and for every character, you had a little
scribble that was unique that the machine could understand and that instead of trying the
machine, trying to teach the machine to recognize human characters, you had basically, they
figured out that it's better and easier to train humans to create human-like characters
that the machine is better at recognizing.
So in the same way, I think what will happen is that humans will be trained to be able
to create the mind pattern that the machine will respond to before the machine truly comprehends
our thoughts.
So the first human brain interfaces will be tricking humans to speak the machine language
where with the right set of electrodes, I can sort of trick my brain into doing this.
And this is the same way that many people teach, like learn to control artificial limbs.
And basically try a bunch of stuff and eventually you figure out how your limbs work.
That might not be very different from how humans learn to use their natural limbs when
they first grow up.
Basically you have these, you know, neoteny period of, you know, this puddle of soup inside
your brain trying to figure out how to even make neural connections before you're born.
And then learning sounds in utero of, you know, all kinds of echoes and, you know, eventually
getting out in the real world.
And I don't know if you've seen newborns, but they just stare around a lot.
You know, one way to think about this as a machine learning person is, oh, they're just
training their edge detectors.
And eventually they figure out how to train their edge detectors.
They work through the second layer of the visual cortex and the third layer and so on
so forth.
And you basically have this learning how to control your limbs that probably comes at
the same time.
You're sort of, you know, throwing random things there and you realize that, oh, wow,
when I do this thing, my limb moves.
Let's do the following experiment.
Take a breath.
What muscles did you flex?
Now take another breath and think what muscles do I flex?
The first thing that you're thinking when you're taking a breath is the impact that
it has on your lungs.
You're like, oh, I'm now going to increase my lungs or I'm not going to bring air in.
But what you're actually doing is just changing your diaphragm.
That's not conscious, of course.
You never think of the diaphragm as a thing.
And why is that?
That's probably the same reason why I think of moving my finger when I actually move my
finger.
I think of the effect instead of actually thinking of whatever muscle is twitching that actually
causes my finger to move.
So we basically, in our first years of life, build up this massive lookup table between
whatever neuronal firing we do and whatever action happens in our body that we control.
If you have a kid grow up with a third limb, I'm sure they'll figure out how to control
them probably at the same rate as their natural limbs.
And a lot of the work would be done by the, so if a third limb is a computer, you kind
of have a, not a faith, but a thought that the brain might be able to figure out, like
the plasticity would come from the brain.
Yeah.
Like the brain would be cleverer than the machine at first.
When I talk about a third limb, that's exactly what I'm saying, an artificial limb that basically
just controls your mouse while you're typing.
You know, perfectly natural thing.
I mean, again, you know, in a few hundred years.
Maybe sooner than that.
But basically, there's, as long as the machine is consistent in the way that it will respond
to your brain impulses, you'll figure out how to control that and you could play tennis
with your third limb.
And let me go back to consistency.
People who have dramatic accidents that basically take out a whole chunk of their brain can
be taught to co-opt other parts of the brain to then control that part.
You can basically build up that tissue again and eventually train your body how to walk
again and how to read again and how to play again and how to think again, how to speak
a language again, et cetera.
So there's a massive amount of malleability that happens, you know, naturally in our way
of controlling our body or brain or thoughts or vocal cords or limbs, et cetera.
And human machine interfaces are all inevitable if we sort of figure out how to read these
electric impulses.
But the resolution at which we can understand human thought right now is nil, is ridiculous.
So how are human thoughts encoded?
It's basically combinations of neurons that co-fire and these create these things called
engrams that eventually form memories and so on and so forth.
We know nothing of all that stuff.
So before we can actually read into your brain that you want to build a program that does
this and this and that, we need a lot of neuroscience.
Well, so to push back on that, do you think it's possible that without understanding the
functionally about the brain or from the neuroscience or the cognitive science or psychology, whichever
level of the brain we look at, do you think we just connect them just like per your previous
point?
If we just have a high enough resolution between connection between Wikipedia and your brain,
the brain will just figure it out without understanding.
Because that's one of the innovations of Neuralink is they're increasing the number of connections
to the brain to like several thousand, which before was, you know, in the dozens or whatever.
You're still off by a few orders of magnetism on the order of seven.
Right.
But the thing is, the hope is if you increase that number more and more and more, maybe you
don't need to understand anything about the actual, how human thought is represented in
the brain.
You can just let it, let it figure it out by itself.
Yeah, Keanu Reeves waking up and saying, I know cuckoo food.
Yeah, exactly.
Yeah.
Exactly.
So yeah, sure.
You don't have faith in the, the plasticity of the brain to that degree.
It's not about brain plasticity.
It's about the input aspect.
Basically, I think on the output aspect, being able to control a machine is something that
you can probably train your neural impulses that you're sending out to sort of match whatever
response you see in the environment.
If this thing moved every single time I thought, a particular thought, then I could figure
out, I could hack my way into moving this thing with just a series of thoughts.
I could think guitar, piano, tennis ball, and then this thing would be moving.
And then, you know, I would just have the series of thoughts that would sort of result
in the impulses that will move this thing the way that I want.
And then eventually it'll become natural because I won't even think about it.
I mean, in the same way that we control our limbs in a very natural way.
But babies don't do that.
Babies have to figure it out.
And you know, some of it is hard-coded, but some of it is actually learned based on whatever
soup of neurons you ended up with, whatever connections you pruned them to, and eventually
you were born with.
You know, a lot of that is coded in the genome, but a huge chunk of that is stochastic instead
of the way that you sort of create all these neurons that migrate, they form connections,
they sort of, you know, spread out, they have particular branching patterns, but then the
connectivity itself, unique in every single new person.
All this to say that on the output side, absolutely, I'm very, very, you know, hopeful
that we can have machines that read thousands of these neuronal connections on the output
side, but on the input side, oh boy, I don't expect any time in the near future we'll be
able to sort of send a series of impulses that will tell me, oh, earth to sun distance,
7.5 million, et cetera, like nowhere.
I mean, I think language will still be the input way rather than sort of any kind of
more complex.
It's a really interesting notion that the ambiguity of language is a feature.
And we evolved for millions of years to take advantage of that ambiguity.
And yet no one teaches us the subtle differences between words that are near cognates and yet
evokes so much more than, you know, one from the other.
And yet, you know, when you're choosing words from a list of 20 synonyms, you know exactly
the connotation of every single one of them.
And that's something that, you know, is there.
So yes, there's ambiguity, but there's all kinds of connotations.
And in the way that we select our words, we have so much baggage that we're sending along,
the way that we're emoting, the way that we're moving our hands every single time we speak,
the, you know, the pauses, the eye contact, et cetera, so much higher bar rate than just
a vocal, you know, string of characters.
Well, let me just take a small tangent on that.
Oh, tangent.
We haven't done that yet.
That's a good idea.
Let's just tangent.
We'll return to the origin of life after.
So I mean, you're Greek, but I'm going on this personal journey.
I'm going to Paris for the explicit purpose of talking to one of the most famous, a couple
who's a famous translators of Russian literature, Dostoevsky Tolstoy.
And they go, that's their art is the translation and everything I've learned about the translation
art, it makes me feel it's so profound in a way that's so much more profound than the
natural language processing papers I read in the machine learning community, that there's
such depth to language that I don't know what to do with.
I don't know if you've experienced that in your own life with knowing multiple languages.
I don't know what to, I don't know how to make sense of it, but there's so much loss
in translation between Russian and English and getting a sense of that.
For example, there's just taking a single sentence from Dostoevsky and there's a lot
of them.
You could talk for hours about how to translate that sentence properly.
That captures the meaning, the period, the culture, the humor, the wit, the suffering
that was in the context of the time, all of that could be a single sentence.
You could talk forever about what it takes to translate that correctly.
I don't know what to do with that.
So being Greek, it's very hard for me to think of a sentence or even a word without going
into the full etymology of that word, breaking up every single atom of that sentence and
every single atom of these words and rebuilding it back up.
I have three kids and the way that I teach them Greek is the same way that the documentary
I was mentioning earlier about understanding the deep roots of all of these words.
It's very interesting that every single time I hear a new word that I've never heard before,
I go and figure out the etymology of that word because I will never appreciate that
word without understanding how it was initially formed.
Interesting.
But how does that help?
Because that's not the full picture.
No, no, of course, of course.
But what I'm trying to say is that knowing the components teaches you about the context
of the formation of that word and sort of the original usage of that word.
And then, of course, the word takes new meaning as you create it from its parts and that meaning
then gets augmented and two synonyms that sort of have different roots will actually
have implications that carry a lot of that baggage of the historical provenance of these
words.
So before working on genome evolution, my passion was evolution of language and sort
of tracing cognates across different languages through their etymologies.
That's fascinating that there's parallels between, I mean, of course, the idea that
there's evolutionary dynamics to our language.
Yeah.
Every single word that you utter, parallels, parallels, what does parallels mean?
Para means side by side, alleles from alleles, which means identical twins, parallels.
I mean, name any word and there's so much baggage, so much beauty in how that word came
to be and how this word took a new meaning than the sum of its parts.
Yeah, and they're just words.
They don't have any physical grounding.
And now you take these words and you weave them into a sentence.
The emotional invocations of that weaving are fathomless.
And all of those emotions all live in the brains of humans.
In the eye of the beholder.
No, seriously, you have to embrace this concept of the eye of the beholder.
It's the conceptualization that nothing takes meaning with one person creating it.
Everything takes meaning in the receiving end.
And the emergent properties of these communication networks where every single, if you look at
the network of ourselves and how they're communicating with each other, every cell has its own code.
This code is modulated by the epigenome.
This creates a bunch of different cell types.
Each cell type now has its own identity, yet they all have the common root of the stem
cells that sort of led to them.
Each of these identities is now communicating with each other.
They take meaning in their interaction.
There's an emergent property that comes from a bunch of cells being together that is not
in any one of the parts.
If you look at neurons communicating, again, these n-grams don't exist in any one neuron.
They exist in the connection, in the combination of neurons.
And the meaning of the words that I'm telling you is empty until it reaches you and it affects
you in a very different way than it affects whoever's listening to this conversation now.
Because of the emotional baggage that I've grown up with, that you've grown up with,
and that they've grown up with.
And that's, I think, the magic of translation.
If you start thinking of translation as just simply capturing that emotional set of reactions
that you evoke, you need a different set of words to evoke that same set of reactions
to a French person than to a Russian person because of the baggage of the culture that
we grew up in.
So basically, you shouldn't find the best word.
And it's a completely different sentence structure that you will need matched to the cultural
context of the target audience that you have.
Yeah, it's, I mean, you're just, I usually don't think about this, but right now there's
this feeling, as a reminder, there's just you and I talking, but there's several hundred
thousand people who will listen to this.
There's some guy in Russia right now running, like in Moscow, listening to us.
There's somebody in India, I guarantee you, there's somebody in China and South America.
There's somebody in Texas, and they all have different emotional baggage.
They probably got angry earlier on about the whole discussion about coronavirus and about
some aspect of it.
Yeah, it's, and there's that network effect.
It's a beautiful thing, and these lateral transfer of information, that's what makes
the collective quote unquote genome of humanity so unique from any other species.
So you somehow miraculously wrapped it back to the very beginning of when we were talking
about the human, the beauty of the human genome.
So I think this is the right time, unless we want to go for a six to eight hour conversation.
We're going to have to talk again, but I think for now, to wrap it up, this is the right
time to talk about the biggest, most ridiculous question of all, meaning of life.
Off mic, you mentioned to me that you had your 42nd birthday, 42nd being a very special,
absurdly special number, and you had to kind of get together with friends to discuss the
meaning of life.
So let me ask you, as a biologist, as a computer scientist, and as a human, what is the meaning
of life?
I've been asking this question for a long time, ever since my 42nd birthday, but well
before that, in even planning the meaning of life symposium, and symposium, symp means
together, posi actually means to drink together, so symposium is actually a drinking party.
Can you actually elaborate about this meaning of life symposium that you put together?
It's like the most genius idea I've ever heard.
So 42 is obviously the answer to life, the universe, and everything from the Hitchhackers
guy to the galaxy.
And as I was turning 42, I've had the theme for every one of my birthdays, when I was
turning 32, it's 1-0-0-0-0 in binary, so I celebrated my 100,000th binary birthday,
and I had the theme of going back 100,000 years, you know, let's dress something in
the last 100,000 years.
Anyway, I've always had these...
It's such an interesting human being, okay, that's awesome.
I've always had these sort of numerology related announcements for my birthday party.
So what came out of that meaning of life symposium is that I basically asked 42 of my colleagues,
42 of my friends, 42 of my collaborators, to basically give 7-minute species on the meaning
of life, each from their perspective.
And I really encourage you to go there, because it's mind-boggling that every single person
said a different answer.
Every single person started with, I don't know what the meaning of life is, but, and
then give this beautifully eloquently answer, eloquently answer.
And they were all different, but they all were consistent with each other and mutually
synergistic and together forming a beautiful view of what it means to be human in many
ways.
Some people talked about the loss of their loved one, their life partner for many, many
years, and how their life changed through that.
Some people talked about the origin of life.
Some people talked about the difference between purpose and meaning.
I'll maybe quote one of the answers, which is this linguistics professor, a friend of
mine at Harvard, who basically said that she was gonna...
She's Greek as well, and she said, I will give a very Pythian answer.
So Pythia was the oracle of Delphi, who would basically give these very cryptic answers,
very short, but interpretable in many different ways.
There was this whole set of priests who were tasked with interpreting what Pythia had said,
and very often you would not get a clean interpretation, but she said, I will be like Pythian and give
you a very short and multiply interpretable answer, but unlike her, I will actually also
give you three interpretations.
And she said, the answer to the meaning of life is become one.
And the first interpretation is, like a child, become one year old with the excitement of
discovering everything about the world.
Second interpretation, in whatever you take on, become one, the first, the best, excel,
drive yourself to perfection for every one of your tasks, and become one when people
are separate, become one, come together, learn to understand each other.
Damn, that's an answer.
And one way to summarize this whole meaning of life symposium is that the very symposium
was illustrating the quest for meaning, which might itself be the meaning of life.
This constant quest for something sublime, something human, something intangible, some
aspect of what defines us as a species and as an individual, both a quest of me as a
person through my own life, but the meaning of life could also be the meaning of all of
life, what is the whole point of life, why life, why life itself, because we've been
talking about the history and evolution of life, but we haven't talked about why life
in the first place, is life inevitable, is life part of physics, does life transcend
physics by fighting against entropy, by compartmentalizing and increasing concentrations rather than
diluting away, is life a distinct entity in the universe beyond the traditional very simple
physical rules that govern gravity and electromagnetism and all of these forces, is life another
force, is there a life force, is there a unique kind of set of principles that emerge, of
course built on top of the hardware of physics, but is it sort of a new layer of software
or a new layer of a computer system?
So that's at the level of big questions.
There's another aspect of gratitude of basically what I like to say is during this pandemic,
I've basically worked from 6 a.m. until 7 p.m. every single day nonstop, including Saturday
and Sunday.
I've basically broken all boundaries of where life, personal life begins and work life ends.
And that has been exhilarating for me, just the intellectual pleasure that I get from
a day of exhaustion, where at the end of the day, my brain is hurting, I'm telling my wife,
wow, I was useful today.
And there's a certain pleasure that comes from feeling useful.
And there's a certain pleasure that comes from feeling grateful.
So I've written this little sort of prayer for my kids to say at bedtime every night,
where they basically say, thank you, God, for all you have given me and give me the strength
to give unto others with the same love that you have given unto me.
Me as a species are so special, the only ones who worry about the meaning of life.
And maybe that's what makes us human.
And what I like to say to my wife and to my students during this pandemic, work extravaganza,
is every now and then they ask me, but how do you do this?
And I'm like, I'm a workaholic.
I love this.
This is me in the most unfiltered way.
The ability to do something useful, to feel that my brain is being used, to interact with
the smartest people on the planet day in, day out, and to help them discover aspects
of the human genome, of the human brain, of human disease and the human condition that
no one has seen before with data that we're capturing that has never been observed.
And that's another aspect which is on the personal life.
Many people say, oh, I'm not going to have kids.
Why bother?
I can tell you as a father, they're missing half the picture, if not the whole picture.
Teaching my kids about my view of the world and watching through their eyes the naivete
with which they start and the sophistication with which they end up.
The understanding that they have of not just the natural world around them, but of me too.
The unfiltered criticism that you get from your own children that knows no bounds of
honesty.
And I've grown components of my heart that I didn't know I had until you sense that fragility
that vulnerability of the children, that immense love and passion, the unfiltered egoism that
we as adults learn how to hide so much better.
It's just this back of emotions that tell me about the raw materials that make a human
being and how these raw materials can be arranged with more sophistication that we learn through
life to become truly human adults.
But there's something so beautiful about seeing that progression between them, the complexity
of the language growing as more neural connections are formed to realize that the hardware is
getting rearranged as their software is getting implemented on that hardware.
But their frontal cortex continues to grow for another 10 years.
There's neural connections that are continuing to form, new neurons that actually get replicated
and formed and it's just incredible that we have these not just you grow the hardware
for 30 years and then you feed it all of the knowledge, no, no, the knowledge is fed throughout
and is shaping these neural connections as they're forming.
So seeing that transformation from either your own blood or from an adopted child is
the most beautiful thing you can do as a human being and it completes you, it completes
that path, that journey, the create life.
Oh, sure, that's that conception, that's easy, but create human life to add the human part
that takes decades of compassion, of sharing, of love and of anger and of impatience and
impatience.
And as a parent, I think I've become a very different kind of teacher because again, I'm
a professor, my first role is to bring adult human beings into a, you know, more mature
level of adulthood where they learn not just to do science, but they learn the process
of discovery and the process of collaboration, the process of sharing, the process of conveying
the knowledge of encapsulating something incredibly complex and sort of giving it up in sort
of bite-sized chunks that the rest of humanity can appreciate.
I tell my students all the time, if you, you know, like when an apple falls, when a tree
falls in the forest and no one's there to listen, has it really fallen?
The same way, you do this awesome research, if you write an impenetrable paper that no
one will understand, it's as if you never did the awesome research.
So conveying of knowledge, conveying this lateral transfer that I was talking about at
the very beginning of sort of humanity and sort of the sharing of information, all of
that has gotten so much more rich by seeing human beings grow in my own home because that
makes me a better parent and that makes me a better teacher and a better mentor to the
nurturing of my adult children, which are my research group.
First of all, beautifully put, connects beautifully to the vertical and the horizontal inheritance
of ideas that we talked about at the very beginning.
I don't think there's a better way to end it on this poetic and powerful note.
Manolos, thank you so much for talking to us.
A huge honor, we'll have to talk again about the origin of life, about epigenetics, epigenomics,
and some of the incredible research you're doing.
Truly an honor.
Thanks so much for talking to us.
Thank you.
Such a pleasure.
It's such a pleasure.
I mean, your questions are outstanding.
I've had such a blast here.
I can't wait to be back.
Awesome.
Thanks for listening to this conversation with Manolos Kellis.
And thank you to our sponsors, Blinkist, Aidsleep, and Masterclass.
Please consider supporting this podcast by going to blinkist.com slash lex, aidsleep.com
slash lex, and masterclass.com slash lex.
Click the links, buy the stuff, get the discount.
It's the best way to support this podcast.
If you enjoy this thing, subscribe on YouTube, review Fast Stars and Apple Podcasts.
Support on Patreon or connect with me on Twitter at Lex Freedman.
And now let me leave you with some words from Charles Darwin that I think Manolos represents
quite beautifully.
If I had my life to live over again, I would have made a rule to read some poetry and listen
to some music at least once every week.
Thank you for listening and hope to see you next time.